Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Epigenetic tuning of brain signal entropy in emergent human social behavior

A Correction to this article was published on 12 September 2020

This article has been updated

Abstract

Background

How the brain develops accurate models of the external world and generates appropriate behavioral responses is a vital question of widespread multidisciplinary interest. It is increasingly understood that brain signal variability—posited to enhance perception, facilitate flexible cognitive representations, and improve behavioral outcomes—plays an important role in neural and cognitive development. The ability to perceive, interpret, and respond to complex and dynamic social information is particularly critical for the development of adaptive learning and behavior. Social perception relies on oxytocin-regulated neural networks that emerge early in development.

Methods

We tested the hypothesis that individual differences in the endogenous oxytocinergic system early in life may influence social behavioral outcomes by regulating variability in brain signaling during social perception. In study 1, 55 infants provided a saliva sample at 5 months of age for analysis of individual differences in the oxytocinergic system and underwent electroencephalography (EEG) while listening to human vocalizations at 8 months of age for the assessment of brain signal variability. Infant behavior was assessed via parental report. In study 2, 60 infants provided a saliva sample and underwent EEG while viewing faces and objects and listening to human speech and water sounds at 4 months of age. Infant behavior was assessed via parental report and eye tracking.

Results

We show in two independent infant samples that increased brain signal entropy during social perception is in part explained by an epigenetic modification to the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and accounts for significant individual differences in social behavior in the first year of life. These results are measure-, context-, and modality-specific: entropy, not standard deviation, links OXTR methylation and infant behavior; entropy evoked during social perception specifically explains social behavior only; and only entropy evoked during social auditory perception predicts infant vocalization behavior.

Conclusions

Demonstrating these associations in infancy is critical for elucidating the neurobiological mechanisms accounting for individual differences in cognition and behavior relevant to neurodevelopmental disorders. Our results suggest that an epigenetic modification to the oxytocin receptor gene and brain signal entropy are useful indicators of social development and may hold potential diagnostic, therapeutic, and prognostic value.

Peer Review reports

Background

Variability is a fundamental property of neural systems at multiple hierarchical levels, from the dynamics of ion channels to the convergence of multiple independent synaptic inputs [1, 2]. Recent work has capitalized on the inherently fluctuating nature of the brain to understand how variability in neural signals, which is often excluded from analysis as mere “noise,” may serve a valuable functional role [1,2,3,4,5]. Neural variability measured via electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) shows strong links to both cognitive performance and cognitive development. Specifically, increased variability is associated with more accurate and stable perceptual, cognitive, and behavioral performance [6,7,8,9,10,11], and variability increases with development from infancy into adulthood [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18]. These associations may occur because the addition of a moderate amount of random noise enhances and more accurately represents an underlying signal [2]. Such variability in brain activity also facilitates the exchange of information between neurons [19, 20], enhancing neural synchrony and promoting the formation of robust, adaptable networks that are not overly reliant on any particular node and display a greater dynamic range [2, 21, 22]. Together, these functions suggest that neural variability may act to appropriately weight incoming information such that important stimuli are maximally salient and enable the most flexible behavioral response.

For humans, social cues are highly important and particularly complex [23]. Social stimuli evoke unique neural and behavioral responses beginning in infancy [24] and require particular flexibility to generate appropriate behavioral responses across multiple contexts. Extensive work has demonstrated that oxytocin, a naturally occurring mammalian hormone, plays an important role in regulating social behavior across species [25], which has been posited to occur through a general effect on basic biological systems that facilitate the detection of and orientation to social information [26]. Oxytocin may facilitate such social signal detection by influencing neural variability in response to social stimuli; oxytocin directly regulates the firing rate of neurons in rodents, enhancing the signal-to-noise ratio, improving information transfer, and balancing neural inhibition and excitation [27,28,29,30]. When synaptic excitation and inhibition are properly balanced, signal variability is optimal and the neural system displays maximum information capacity, information transmission, and dynamic range [3, 19, 31,32,33,34].

We tested the hypothesis that individual differences in the endogenous oxytocinergic system early in life may influence infant social behavior by regulating variability in brain signaling during social perception in two independent studies. In study 1, infants provided a saliva sample at 5 months of age for analysis of individual differences in the oxytocinergic system and underwent EEG while listening to human vocalizations at 8 months of age for the assessment of brain signal variability. Infant behavior was assessed using the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R), a widely used and validated measure of 14 domains of infant behavior and temperament based on parental report [35] that has been shown to correlate with overt behaviors [36,37,38] and to prospectively predict the development of subsequent traits [39,40,41,42]. In study 2, 4-month-old infants provided a saliva sample for analysis of individual differences in the oxytocinergic system and underwent EEG while viewing faces and objects and listening to human speech and water sounds. Infant behavior was assessed using the Short Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-RS) [43] and a dynamic social interaction eye-tracking paradigm.

To assay individual differences in the human oxytocinergic system, we examined an epigenetic modification to the DNA of the oxytocin receptor gene, OXTR methylation, at cytosine-phosphate-guanine (CpG) site -934 (hg38_chr3:8,769,121-8,769,122). While currently available peripheral measures of the oxytocin peptide itself remain unreliable [44], this assay of its receptor is reliable, stable, and specific [45]. We have previously assayed methylation levels from all CpG sites within two OXTR CpG islands and shown that the level of DNA methylation at CpG site -934 is (1) significantly negatively associated with gene expression in human cortex [46], suggesting a regulatory role in gene transcription; (2) highly variable in the general population and associated with neural response during social perceptual tasks in neurotypical adults [47,48,49], suggesting it is a viable marker of individual differences in (endo) phenotypes; and (3) elevated in the brain and blood of both individuals with autism [46] and vole pups who experienced lower parental care early in life [50], suggesting this marker is indicative of individual developmental differences that are reflected in both causative (brain) and peripheral (blood) tissue.

We employed two methods for quantifying brain signal variability: standard deviation (SD) to measure overall distributional width (variance) of the signal and multiscale entropy (MSE) [51] to measure signal irregularity across temporal scales. Entropy tests for repeating patterns in a time series and assigns a high value to irregular signals and a low value to ordered, predictable signals. MSE is a popular technique in neuroscience because the brain is a complex system that operates on multiple time scales. In MSE, the time series is coarse grained (downsampled), and entropy is computed on the coarse-grained time series. When applied to brain signal data, MSE provides insight into the time-structure and linearity/nonlinearity of fluctuations in neural activity and network dynamics [52]. Entropy at short time scales captures both fast and slow oscillations and is thought to represent processing within local networks, whereas entropy at longer time scales captures slower oscillations and is thought to represent the integration of widely distributed cortical networks [52, 53].

Both SD [11, 54,55,56] and MSE [7, 8, 14, 15, 17, 57] have been positively associated with developmental and behavioral outcomes. However, no study to date has directly compared the explanatory power of these two variability measures, nor considered a role for oxytocinergic system function as an underlying molecular mechanism capable of contributing to brain signal variability during social perception in humans.

Early development is thought to constitute a period of increased sensitivity to the regulatory effects of epigenetic mechanisms [58], and infancy represents a particularly sensitive period in early development characterized by dramatic changes in brain structure and function [59]. Investigating specific epigene-brain-behavior associations early in development as done in the current studies can therefore be seen as a critical step in elucidating the mechanisms contributing to individual differences in social development.

Methods

Tissue comparison study

DNA collection and isolation

To determine the reliability of OXTR methylation values obtained from saliva, we first performed a tissue comparison study in which 207 healthy Caucasian adults (114 females) aged 16 to 81 years (M = 37.74, SD = 22.95) provided 5-mL passive drool in a Falcon 50 mL Conical Centrifuge Tube (Fisher Scientific, Hampton, NH) for assessment of saliva methylation and 8-mL blood in either mononuclear cell preparation tubes (BD Biosciences, Franklin Lanes, NJ) for assessment of peripheral blood mononuclear cell (PBMC) methylation (n = 142), or PAXgene Blood DNA Tubes (Qiagen, Valencia, CA) for assessment of whole blood methylation (n = 182). One hundred seventeen participants provided all three sample types. Saliva cells were pelleted in 20 mL 1x phosphate-buffered saline (Life Technologies, Carlsbad, CA) by centrifuging at 1800 rcf for 5 min. Pellets were then transferred into a microcentrifuge tube and frozen at − 20 °C prior to DNA extraction. We isolated DNA from saliva cells using reagents supplied in the QIAamp DNA Blood Mini Kit (Qiagen, Valencia, CA) following Qiagen’s Supplemental Protocol for Isolation of Genomic DNA from Saliva. We isolated DNA from peripheral blood mononuclear cells using reagents and protocol supplied in the Gentra Puregene Blood Kit (Qiagen, Valencia, CA). We isolated DNA from whole blood using reagents and protocol supplied in the PAXgene Blood DNA Kit (Qiagen, Valencia, CA).

Epigenetic analysis

Bisulfite conversion, polymerase chain reaction, and pyrosequencing were performed in triplicate exactly as detailed in Puglia et al. [48]. On average, replicates deviated from the mean ± 1.66%. PBMC methylation levels averaged 47.39% (SD = 6.42), whole blood methylation values averaged 46.61% (SD = 8.11), and saliva methylation values averaged 45.33% (SD = 6.46).

Study 1

Participants

We report on a previously-acquired longitudinal dataset in which 81 healthy Caucasian infants (41 females) provided a saliva sample at 5 months of age (M = 149.93 days, SD = 14.29) and at 8 months of age (M = 247.54 days, SD = 5.87), underwent EEG, and received parent-reported behavioral ratings. All parents provided written informed consent prior to the study, approved by the Leipzig University Ethics Committee. Families were compensated with travel money, an infant t-shirt, a printed photo of the infant, and a toy for participation.

DNA collection and isolation

Passive drool was collected from infants at 5 months of age using CS-2 sponges and OG-250 kits (DNA Genotek, Ottawa, Canada) and was stored at room temperature until DNA isolation. We incubated collection kits at 50 °C for 1 h, then centrifuged for 10 min at 200 rcf to release all liquid from sponges. We isolated DNA from 500 μL of saliva using the manual purification protocol from DNA Genotek. DNA was stored in Hydration Solution (10 mM Tris, 1 mM EDTA, pH 7–8, Qiagen, Valencia, CA) and quantitated using nanodrop. One infant was excluded from the final analysis due to insufficient DNA.

Epigenetic analysis

Bisulfite conversion, polymerase chain reaction, and pyrosequencing were performed in triplicate exactly as in the tissue comparison study. On average, replicates deviated from the mean ± 1.83%. Infant saliva methylation levels averaged 40.34% (SD = 4.69).

EEG acquisition and preprocessing

Infants participated in an EEG paradigm in which they were presented with 4-s auditory clips of human vocalizations (infants crying, infants laughing, and neutral adult hummed speech). Stimuli were separated by a random inter-stimulus interval of 1000+ ms. Throughout the procedure, infants were seated on their parent’s lap in a dimly lit, sound-attenuated, and electrically shielded room. Infants were simultaneously presented with a non-social screensaver displaying animated bubbles to facilitate attention retention. Parents listened to classical music presented via headphones and were instructed not to talk to or interact with their infant during the course of the experiment. The EEG session ended when the infant became fussy or inattentive. EEG was acquired from 27 electrodes affixed to an elastic cap (EasyCap CmbH, Germany). Additional acquisition information is detailed in [60].

EEG preprocessing was completed using EEGLab, v14.1.1 [61], and progressed as detailed in prior infant [60] and MSE [8, 15] EEG studies. Data were bandpass filtered 0.3 to 20 Hz to remove slow drift and muscle artifacts [60], re-referenced to the average of all scalp electrodes [8, 15], and segmented into stimulus-evoked epochs 100 ms before stimulus onset to 1000 ms post stimulus onset with pre-baseline correction [60]. To assess spontaneous, ongoing variability, we randomly extracted 1100-ms epochs from the inter-stimulus interval that were not time-locked to the onset of a stimulus and did not overlap with stimulus-evoked epochs. Epochs contaminated with excessive amplitude standard deviations (> 100 μV in ocular channels, > 80 μV in scalp electrodes) within a 200-ms sliding window were discarded as artifacts [60]. Participants with at least 30 artifact-free auditory-evoked trials (10 from each social-auditory condition) and 30 artifact-free randomly sampled ongoing trials (n = 58) were retained in the analysis. This rejection rate (27.5%) can be compared to that reported in a meta-analysis of 149 infant EEG studies that found an average rejection rate of 49.2% [62].

We then completed an independent components analysis (ICA) on all remaining concatenated trials to remove components with clear ocular, muscular, or electrical artifacts identified via manual inspection of component topography, frequency, and time course [15]. On average, 4.43 (range 2 to 10) components were removed. The number of components removed did not correlate with MSE (r = .17, p = .171) or SD (r = −.08, p = .519) metrics, and removed components did not show significant event-related potential (ERP) effects (Table 1).

Table 1 ERP replication results

Because the number of data points included in MSE calculation can influence reliability of the estimates [63], we selected the 30 auditory-evoked (10 from each social-auditory condition) and 30 spontaneous-ongoing trials with total global field power (GFP) [8] closest to the median GFP for each participant for inclusion in downstream analyses (16,470 data points per condition). This selection criterion identifies the most representative trials for each individual and minimizes the influence of trials potentially contaminated with high residual amplitude artifacts [8]. To ensure that the current procedures did not obscure or eliminate relevant evoked response, we reproduced all results from Missana et al. [60] using these data re-referenced to the average of the mastoids (Table 1).

Brain signal variability analyses

After preprocessing, we subjected these thirty 1100-ms auditory-evoked epochs and thirty 1100-ms spontaneous-ongoing epochs to brain signal variability analyses. Brain signal variability can be quantified in many ways [3]. Here we consider two of the most commonly applied measures of brain signal variability—SD, a measure of signal variance, and MSE, a measure of temporal irregularity.

Multiscale entropy

MSE [51] computation involves (1) coarse graining the time series to scale s by averaging together s successive, non-overlapping points, and (2) computing sample entropy [64]. Sample entropy is a measure of signal irregularity in which two patterns of m length are considered indistinguishable if each point k is within k ± r. Then, the natural log of the ratio of the count of m patterns to the count of m + 1 patterns is computed. Higher sample entropy values therefore indicate higher irregularity in the data because patterns of length m + 1 reoccur less often than patterns of length m.

In Costa’s original MSE algorithm [51], r is calculated as a percentage of SD of the original time series and remains constant across all scales. However, this method conflates entropy with variance [63, 65]. We therefore used a modified algorithm [65] that recalculates r at each scale as a percentage of SD of the coarse-grained time series. We computed MSE on the residuals of the EEG signal (i.e., after subtracting the within-person average response across the 30 trials within each condition) for the 30 auditory-evoked and 30 spontaneous-ongoing trials for each scalp electrode using the algorithm described in Grandy et al. [63] for estimating MSE across discontinuous segments, modified to recalculate r for each scale. Trials from all social auditory conditions were combined in the auditory-evoked analysis to provide sufficient data points for accurate MSE estimation [63]. Parameter values were set to pattern length m  =  2 and similarity criterion r  =  .5, the most commonly used and recommended values ([52, 63], see also [63] which found no substantial effect on the accuracy and precision of MSE estimates across multiple m and r parameter settings).

Here, we focused our primary analyses on the area under the multiscale entropy curve (MSEAUC) for each of the 27 electrodes for scales 1 to 100 (corresponding to 500 to 5 Hz) to obtain a comprehensive picture of the temperodynamic structure of our data. Average MSE curves are plotted in Fig. 1, and average area under the curve values are listed in Table 2. We also considered entropy at scales 1 (MSE1, 500 Hz), 50 (MSE50, 10 Hz), and 100 (MSE100, 5 Hz) to assess the impact of specific time scales on our models in secondary analyses. We found scales 37 to 60 (8 to 13 Hz) show the highest correlation with MSEAUC across all electrodes, suggesting these scales, in particular, may drive our results with MSEAUC.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Group average multiscale entropy curves. Study 1 average multiscale entropy curves from for scales 1 to 100 (500 to 5 Hz) are plotted for the social evoked (left) and ongoing (right) EEG signal for each electrode (n = 55)

Table 2 Average area under the multiscale entropy curve values

Standard deviation analysis

To capture moment-to-moment variance in the EEG signal, we calculated SD on the residuals of the EEG signal (i.e., after subtracting the within-person average ERP) for the 30 auditory-evoked and 30 spontaneous-ongoing trials for each of the 27 electrodes using two methods commonly reported in the literature. Specifically, SD can be calculated across the continuous time series as a measure of distributional width (SDCONT, the focus of our primary analyses) (e.g., [66]), or across trials as a measure of the trial-by-trial reliability of the evoked response (SDTXT) (e.g., [54]). These computation methods yield highly correlated values (all rs ≥ .99). To equalize data volume and computation across our brain signal variability metrics, we also consider the area under the course-grained SD curve (SDAUC), and SD at scales 1 (SD1, equivalent to SDCONT), 50 (SD50), and 100 (SD100) in secondary analyses.

Finally, because entropy explicitly incorporates signal SD when defining the similarity criterion parameter r, r is larger for a signal with greater SD, meaning the entropy algorithm is more likely to identify matches, resulting in a lower entropy value [67]. However, only MSE is sensitive to temporal dependencies in a signal (Fig. 2). To understanding the unique contribution of entropy and variance of a signal on our models, we orthogonalized MSE and SD estimates by regressing SD from MSE scale-wise for each participant, electrode, and condition prior to computing the area under the curve (AUC) in secondary analyses (MSESDRes).

Fig. 2
figure 2

An illustration of the temporal dependency of multiscale entropy. a We created a surrogate time series (red) by randomly shuffling segments of the original time series (black) consisting of actual EEG data from one trial. The standard deviations of the original and surrogate time series are equivalent, 22.63. b We find higher entropy for the surrogate time series (red) than the original time series (black) across time scales because the scrambling procedure introduced greater temporal irregularity into the surrogate time series

Infant behavior

Infant behavior was assessed via parental report with the IBQ-R [35], a 191-item measure organized into 14 subscales that is designed to assess infant behavior and temperament from 3 to 12 months of age. Parents rate how frequently their infant displayed specific behaviors on a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always).

To separately consider social and non-social aspects of infant behavior, we created new Social and Non-Social IBQ-R constructs. We first manually identified unambiguously social and non-social items by subscale. Then, we conducted an item analysis to ensure that included items constituted a valid measure of each construct using Cronbach’s α, a measure of internal consistency that determines the correlation among related items, implemented in the psych package in R [68, 69]. Specifically, we removed items until Cronbach’s α > .70 [70] for all Social and Non-Social subscales or until the removal of additional items did not improve α. Subscales that did not achieve α > .60 were not considered further. Models 2 and 3 were run with Social and Non-Social constructs both before and after item removal. Results did not appreciably change after item removal; results are presented after item removal. Finally, we tested for construct validity to confirm that the Social and Non-Social subscales were identified as unique, distinguishable constructs in the model by confirming that the loadings for each subscale exceeded .5 [71] for its own construct (Social IBQ-R M = .55, Non-Social IBQ-R: M = .68) and that the cross-loadings for each subscale did not exceed .5 onto the other construct (Social IBQ-R: M = .02, Non-Social IBQ-R: M = − .01). Individual items included in the final Social and Non-Social IBQ-R constructs are listed in Table 3.

Table 3 Social and Non-Social IBQ constructs

Experimental design and statistical analysis

We employed a multivariate, prediction-based approach to develop models of our hypothesized epigene-brain-behavior associations with partial least squares path modeling (PLS-PM). PLS-PM is a statistical method for studying complex multivariate associations among measured and latent variables [71]. A path model consists of an inner model, in which associations (path coefficients) are calculated between constructs (latent variables, a predictor points to a criterion), and an outer model, in which associations (loadings) are calculated between the original measured variables (indicators) and their latent variables. Factor scores are calculated for each observation as a weighted average of the measured variables.

Particularly given the novelty of the present research, PLS is better suited than other multivariate techniques like covariance-based structural equation modeling (CBSEM) because PLS is considered optimal for exploratory, prediction-based research where theory is less developed [71, 72]. Complex models with many observed variables and relationships can be estimated with smaller sample sizes with PLS than required by CBSEM [72]. Unlike CBSEM, PLS is a nonparametric technique which makes no assumptions about the normality of the distribution of the data. Furthermore, PLS is well suited to the highly dimensional, highly correlated nature of neuroimaging data (i.e., among many electrodes, voxels) [73]. For these reasons, PLS has become a popular and commonly used modeling technique within neuroimaging [3, 7,8,9, 11, 15, 66, 74].

Model specification and assessment

Traditionally, it is assumed that there is a system of linear associations between latent variables. However, to account for potential curvilinear [75,76,77] associations among our epigene-brain-behavior variables, we estimated all models using WarpPLS v6.0, the only software currently available to explicitly identify nonlinear functions connecting pairs of latent variables [78].

For all models, we estimated a reflexive outer model for all constructs using the PLS regression algorithm with WarpPLS. Methylation values generated in each of the 3 replicate analyses served as indicators for the epigene (OXTR methylation) latent variable, data from each of the 27 electrodes served as indicators for the brain (MSE, SD) latent variables, and scores from each subscale served as indicators for the behavior (IBQ-R, 14; Social IBQ-R, 7; Non-social IBQ-R, 6) latent variables. Inner model path coefficients were estimated using the Warp2 algorithm, which tests for second-order polynomial associations among latent variables through best-fitting nonlinear u-curve or j-curve functions (e.g., logarithmic, hyperbolic decay, exponential decay, exponential, quadratic relationships, etc.) that minimize sums of squared residuals on a bivariate basis. These functions are identified through the following equation, where F1(LVp1) and F2(LVp2) are nonlinear functions that relate blocks of predictor latent variables (LVp1, LVp2) to a criterion latent variable (LVc), p1 and p2 are the path coefficients, and E is the error term [78, 79]:

$$ LVc=p1\ast F1(LVp1)+p2\ast F2(LVp2)+E $$

If the algorithm determines that a curvilinear relationship best fits the distribution of points associated with a pair of latent variables, WarpPLS first performs nonlinear transformations on the factor scores and then calculates the path coefficient between latent variables, assigning the sign of the coefficient to be the sign of the correlation between latent variables prior to the nonlinear transformation [78, 79]. If the algorithm determines that a linear or quasilinear relationship best fits the distribution of points associated with a pair of latent variables, WarpPLS calculates the path coefficient between latent variables without performing nonlinear transformations [78, 79]. The best-fitting curves are representations of the identified functions. However, when high measurement error exists in the dataset, there is increased likelihood that the identified functions are distorted by this error and do not represent the true underlying functions. To determine whether the nonlinear functions discovered by WarpPLS represent good approximations of the true underlying functions, one can divide the dataset into quantiles and plot the mean value of each criterion latent variable for each quantile. If visual inspection of these quantile plots mimics the shape of the best-fitting curve, one can conclude that the identified function is a good representation of the true underlying function [78], p. 98. Quartile plots for all significant effects for each model can be viewed in Additional file 1.

Outliers

After initial model fitting, values were considered outliers if the factor score fell more than 3 median absolute deviations from the median. We selected this relatively conservative criterion to balance outlier detection with subject retention. We first determined whether these outliers were driven by a single indicator within blocks. Methylation values for two subjects were identified as outliers in single replicates. These outlier replicates were removed and imputed with the mean of the other two replicate values for these subjects. One infant was identified as an outlier across all three replicates; one infant was identified as an outlier in both MSE and SD factor scores; one infant was identified as an outlier in the behavioral factor scores but was not an outlier in any single behavioral indicator. These three participants were removed, and models were re-estimated. Results did not appreciably change with or without outliers; we therefore conservatively report on models excluding outliers (n = 55, 29 females).

After removal of outliers, we identified indicators with negative loadings and reverse-coded these items. For models with original IBQ-R subscores, these items included Activity Level, Distress to Limitations, Fear, Perceptual Sensitivity, and Sadness. For models with Social/Non-Social IBQ-R subscores, these items included Social Fear and Non-Social Fear.

Next, we checked for convergent and discriminant validity by identifying and removing any items that did not significantly load onto its construct or that loaded higher onto another construct for each model. These included Duration of Orientation, Perceptual Sensitivity, Vocal Reactivity, Social Duration of Orientation, Non-Social Fear, Non-Social Duration of Orientation, and Non-Social High Pleasure. Removing these items did not appreciably change results; results are presented with these items removed.

Finally, we determined that the square root of the average variance extracted (AVE) was greater than the correlations between constructs. We report construct internal consistency and reliability as indexed through the composite reliability coefficient (recommended value > .60, [80]), and explanatory power through R2 values in Table 4 for each model. For all significant effects, we report path coefficients (β), standard errors for path coefficients (SE), p values (p) estimated with delete-1 jackknifing, and effect sizes analogous to Cohen’s f2 coefficient [81]. WarpPLS calculates effect sizes as the absolute values of the individual contributions of the corresponding predictor latent variables to the R2 coefficients of the criterion latent variable in each latent variable block [82]. The reported f2 statistics can be interpreted as recommended by Cohen [81]—.02, .15, and .35 indicate small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively.

Table 4 Model quality indices

The oxytocinergic system, social behavior, and their related brain systems have all been shown to be sexually dimorphic [25]. We therefore tested for sex effects in our models by examining differences in path coefficients across male and females using multi-group analysis with pooled standard error in WarpPLS.

Sample size

After preprocessing and outlier removal, the final sample consisted of 55 participants with complete epigenetic, neural, and behavioral data. To determine that we had sufficient power for our models with this sample size, we followed the recommendation of Chin and Newsted [83] and computed a power analysis based on the portion of the model with the largest number of predictors. In our models, IBQ-R constructs have the largest number of predictors—up to 3. An extensive literature review suggests a moderate association (r = .3 to .5) between measures of neural variability and behavioral outcomes [8, 9, 11]. A multiple regression power analysis [84] determined that 56 participants are needed to detect an effect size (ρ2) of 0.25 with 3 predictors, 95% power, and α = .05.

Study 2

Participants

Sixty-five (31 female) infants were recruited from the greater Charlottesville area to provide a saliva sample, undergo EEG and eye tracking, and receive parent-reported behavioral ratings at 4 months of age (M = 131.92 ± 11.59 days). The primary caregiver accompanied the infant to all appointments and provided written informed consent for a protocol approved by the University of Virginia Health and Human Sciences Institutional Review Board. Families were paid $50 for their participation. Eleven infants (4 female) returned within 1 week (M = 5.23 ± 2.00 days) to assess the test-retest reliability of brain signal entropy in infancy.

The target sample size for study 2 was determined via a power analysis using effect sizes established in study 1 and following recommendations of Chin and Newsted [83]. In our study 2 model, behavioral constructs have the largest number of predictors—3. A two-tailed multiple regression power analysis [84] determined that 58 participants are needed to detect an effect size of 0.29 with 3 predictors, 95% power, and α = .05.

The target sample size for the test-retest reliability analysis was determined via power analysis tables provided by Bujang and Baharum [85] which specify that 10 subjects are sufficient to detect an interclass correlation coefficient (ICC) of .70 based on two observations with 80% power.

DNA collection and epigenetic analysis

Passive drool collection, DNA isolation, and epigenetic analysis were performed exactly as described in study 1. On average, replicates deviated from the mean ± 2.70%. Methylation levels averaged 42.02% (SD = 4.53).

EEG acquisition and preprocessing

Infants participated in an EEG paradigm (Fig. 3) consisting of four conditions, resulting in a 2 × 2 design with the factors context (social or non-social) and modality (visual or auditory). Visual social stimuli were obtained from the Amsterdam Dynamic Facial Expression Set [86] and consisted of six female actors each turning towards or away from the camera to the left or right and then smiling for a total of 24 videos. Visual non-social stimuli consisted of dynamic color videos of common objects (e.g., vegetables, toys) rotating. Stimuli were obtained from the Amsterdam Library of Object Images [87], a database of objects photographed in multiple viewing directions. Objects were first cropped and placed on a background matching the social stimuli, then selected such that the non-social stimulus set were matched to the social stimulus set on luminance (Msocial = 177.16, Mnon-social = 178.24, t = 0.13, p = 0.898, contrast (Msocial = 52.95, Mnon-social = 53.73, t = 0.16, p = 0.875), and spatial frequency (Msocial = 13,743.58, Mnon-social = 12,067.46, t = − 1.92, p = 0.068) using custom MATLAB scripts adopted from the SHINE [88] toolbox. The final set consisted of 12 unique objects, each rotating to the left and to the right for a total of 24 videos. Each visual trial had a total duration of 18 s and consisted of six unique 2400-ms videos presented at a visual angle of 8° in a randomized order and with a randomized inter-stimulus interval ranging from 500 to 1000 ms. White noise generated in MATLAB was presented as auditory stimuli during visual trials.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Example stimuli from the study 2 EEG paradigm. The EEG paradigm had a 2 × 2 design with the factors context (social or non-social) and modality (visual or auditory). Visual social stimuli consisted of videos of women turning their heads and smiling. Visual non-social stimuli consisted of videos of common objects rotating. During visual perception, white noise was played in the auditory modality. Auditory social stimuli consisted of infant-directed speech. Auditory non-social stimuli consisted of recordings of water sounds. During auditory perception, a video of static-like salt and pepper noise was played in the visual modality

Auditory non-social stimuli consisted of sounds of water from nature (e.g., rain, surf) and household products (e.g., bubbling, splashing) downloaded from http://www.findsounds.com. Auditory social stimuli consisted of naturalistic infant-directed speech recorded from seven English-speaking mothers as they spoke to their preverbal children in their homes [89] downloaded from the Child Language Data Exchange System [90]. Clips containing single-word utterances (e.g., “shoes,” “open,” “hot”) or short phrases (e.g., “oh my goodness,” “uh oh,” “bye-bye”) were extracted from the recordings. Clips containing incoherent speech or background noises were discarded, and individual clips were selected such that the social stimulus set matched the non-social stimulus set on mean fundamental frequency (Msocial = 311.16, Mnon-social = 335.29, t = − 1.29, p = .200), standard deviation of fundamental frequency (Msocial = 66.85, Mnon-social = 57.82, t = 0.95, p = .343), and duration (Msocial = 0.87, Mnon-social = 0.97, t = − 1.35, p = .179) using Praat v6.0.36 [91] and custom MATLAB scripts. The final auditory stimulus set consisted of 60 unique social and 60 unique non-social auditory clips. Clips were grouped by condition (social/non-social) into six 10-clip, 18-s trials such that no word or water sound type repeated within a trial. The inter-stimulus interval between clips ranged from 500 to 1000 ms, randomized across participants. The order of clips within a trial and the presentation order of trials was randomized across participants. Static-like salt and pepper noise videos generated in MATLAB were presented as visual stimuli during auditory blocks.

Stimuli were presented with PsychToolBox v3.0.14 [92] in MATLAB. Infants were seated on their caregiver’s lap approximately 100 cm from a computer monitor throughout the experiment. Caregivers were instructed not to talk or interact with the infant during the course of the experiment. Trials within a block were pseudo-randomized such that visual and auditory trials alternated. A contracting and expanding colorful shape paired with an attention-getting sound was presented within the inter-trial interval to regain the infant’s attention to the center of the computer screen, at which point the experimenter initiated the beginning of the next block. The experimenter viewed the infant via live stream from the control area and could pause the experiment between trials to regain the infant’s attention or compliance if necessary. The EEG session ended when the infant became fussy or inattentive or after 24 blocks. On average, infants completed 7.03 (SD = 3.00) blocks of the paradigm.

EEG was recorded from 32 Ag/AgCl active actiCAP slim electrodes (Brain Products GmbH, Germany) affixed to an elastic cap using the 10–20 electrode placement system. The horizontal electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded from two electrodes (F7, F8), which are part of the cap located at the outer canthi of both eyes. The vertical EOG was recorded from two electrodes (FP1, FP2), which are part of the cap on the supraorbital ridge of both eyes. The infant’s head circumference was first obtained to determine the correct cap size prior to capping and gel application. Impedances were assessed via the actiCAP Control Box prior to recording. EEG was amplified with a BrainAmp DC Amplifier and recorded using BrainVision Recorder software with a sampling rate of 5000 Hz, online referenced to FCz, and online band-pass filtered between 0.1 and 1000 Hz. Data were downsampled to 500 Hz prior to subsequent analysis.

EEG preprocessing was performed as in study 1. Visual stimuli were segmented into stimulus-evoked epochs 100 ms pre-stimulus onset to 1000 ms post-stimulus onset with pre-baseline correction. Auditory stimuli were segmented into stimulus-evoked epochs 100 ms pre-stimulus onset to 500 ms post-stimulus onset with pre-baseline correction. Artifact rejection criteria were identical to those in study 1. Participants with at least 20 artifact-free visual segments and 40 artifact-free auditory segments were retained in the analyses. Four participants (6.15%) were excluded for an insufficient number of artifact-free segments, and EEG data failed to save due to a technical error for one additional infant in study 2.

We then completed ICA as in study 1 to remove artifactual components. On average, 3.48 (range 2 to 8) components were removed. The number of components removed did not correlate with MSE within the visual (r = .13, p = .330) or auditory (r = − .15, p = .249) modality and did not show significant ERP effects. To include an equivalent number of data points for each condition in the MSE computation, we selected the 20 visual segments (10 social) and the 40 auditory segments (20 social) with a total GFP [8] closest to the median GFP for each participant for inclusion in downstream analyses (5000 data points per condition).

Brain signal variability analyses

MSE was calculated exactly as described in study 1. We consider MSEAUC for each of the 32 electrodes for scales 1 to 50 (corresponding to 500 to 10 Hz).

Test-retest reliability was assessed for average MSEAUC across all conditions via interclass correlation coefficient (ICC). ICC estimates were calculated using the irr v0.84.1 [93] statistical package in R based on a single rating (k = 1), absolute-agreement, 2-way mixed-effects model [94]. EEG data for one infant were excluded due to an insufficient number of artifact-free segments at re-test.

Infant behavior

To separately consider how auditory- and visually evoked MSE impacts domain-specific social behaviors, we considered infant social vocal and visual behaviors independently. Infant vocalization behavior was assessed via the 7-item vocalization subscale of the IBQ-RS [43] in which the parent rates how frequently the infant vocalizes during specific activities on a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always). IBQ-RS data were unavailable for 4 infants. These missing data were imputed via arithmetic mean.

To assess infant visual attention to social information, infants participated in an eye-tracking paradigm in which they viewed 16-s videos of children playing and engaging in nonverbal communication in a naturalistic environment. Stimuli were provided at the courtesy of researchers from the Center for Autism Research at the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia and included 16 silent video clips of 8 sibling pairs of school-aged children playing with various toys (see [95] for stimulus acquisition details). Videos were presented to the infants in a randomized order. Between each video, infants’ attention was re-oriented to the center of the screen with a colorful spinning object and was paired with an attention-getting sound. Once the infant fixated on the attention-getter for 500 ms, the next video began.

Stimuli were presented using PsychToolBox v3.0.14 [92] for MATLAB. Eye tracking was recorded with Tobii Pro SDK v1.6 for MATLAB and a Tobii X60 eye tracker mounted to a 17-in. computer screen. The infant was seated on the caregiver’s lap throughout the protocol. The caregiver wore darkened glasses to ensure the eye tracker registered only the infant’s pupils and was instructed not to talk or interact with the infant during the course of the experiment. The infant was first positioned 60 cm from the eye tracker and screen. Then, the infant underwent a 5-point calibration procedure in which a colorful, dynamic object expanded and contracted and was paired with an attention-getting sound. If any points failed to calibrate, the calibration procedure was repeated up to two times for those points at which point the eye-tracking paradigm commenced.

Gaze data preprocessing was carried out using custom MATLAB scripts following Tobii recommendations [96], including gap fill-in using linear interpolation for gaps up to 75 ms, average eye computation from binocular data, and median filtering with a length of 7 points (100 ms) [97]. Dynamic areas of interest (AOIs) were drawn around the faces and provided by researchers at the Center for Autism Research at the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia (see [95] for details).

Social visual attention was defined as the Proportion of Total Fixation Duration to Faces relative to Total Fixation Duration to the entire screen. Calculating a proportional fixation duration is a common technique in infant eye tracking as it accounts for individual differences in overall looking behavior and does not require the implementation of an exclusionary gaze time threshold that would reduce sample size and may produce selection biases. We also calculated the Average Time to First Fixation to Faces to assess how quickly social information captured infant’s attention and Total Fixation Count to Faces to assess the extent of visual exploration of social information. On average, infants completed 8.06 (SD = 4.12) blocks of the paradigm. Eye-tracking data was not available for 8 infants due to a failure of the eye tracker to register the infant’s eyes or technical error. These missing data were imputed via arithmetic mean.

Experimental design and statistical analysis

We employed PLS-PM using WarpPLS as described in study 1. Methylation values generated in each of the 3 replicate analyses served as indicators for the epigene (OXTR methylation) latent variable, Social–Non-social MSEAUC difference scores for each of the 32 electrodes in the auditory and visual modalities served as indicators for the brain (Auditory MSEAUC, Visual MSEAUC) latent variables, scores from each of the 7 items that constitute the IBQ-RS vocalization subscore served as indicators for the Verbal Behavior latent variable, and the three metrics of social visual attention from the eye-tracking paradigm (Average Time to First Fixation to Faces, Total Fixation Count to Faces, and Proportion of Total Fixation Duration to Faces) served as indicators for the Visual Behavior latent variable.

Outliers were defined as in study 1. The methylation value for one subject was identified as an outlier in a single replicate. This outlier replicate was removed and imputed with the mean of the other two replicate values for this subject. Next, we checked for convergent and discriminant validity by identifying and removing any items that did not significantly load onto its construct or that loaded higher onto another construct for each model. These included T8, TP10, and T9 for Auditory MSEAUC, F8 and T8 for Visual MSEAUC, and Average Time to First Fixation for Visual Behavior. Finally, we determined that the square root of the average variance extracted (AVE) was greater than the correlations between constructs. We report construct internal consistency and reliability as indexed through the composite reliability coefficient (recommended value > .60 [80]), and explanatory power through R2 values in Table 4 for this model.

Results

Tissue comparison study

Saliva is a reliable tissue for assaying OXTR methylation

Previous work has shown that OXTR methylation assayed from peripheral blood at CpG site -934 reflects the level of DNA methylation at this site in the brain [46, 50]—the causal tissue for behavior. To expand this marker for use with infants, we first established that OXTR methylation levels in saliva, a peripheral tissue more appropriate for vulnerable populations, correspond to OXTR methylation levels in blood. Healthy adults provided both passive drool and intravenous blood samples for assessment of whole blood (n = 182) and/or PBMC (n = 142) methylation. Epigenetic analyses revealed significant correlations between OXTR methylation derived from saliva and whole blood (r(180) = .75 [95% confidence interval .68, .81], p < .001), and saliva and PBMC (r(140) = .78, [.70, .83], p < .001) at site -934 (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Saliva is a reliable tissue for assaying OXTR methylation. DNA methylation values at OXTR cytosine-phosphate-guanine (CpG) site -934 are significantly correlated between (a) saliva and peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMC) (n = 142, r(140) = .78, p < .001), and (b) saliva and whole blood (n = 182, r(180) = .75, p < .001)

Study 1

Brain signal entropy is associated with OXTR methylation and accounts for individual differences in infant behavior

Brain signal variability can be quantified in different ways. Two of the most commonly examined measures are entropy and SD. This study is the first to directly compare the explanatory power of these measures within one model. In our primary analyses, we consider the area under the MSE curve (MSEAUC) for scales 1 to 100 (corresponding to 500 to 5 Hz) to obtain a comprehensive picture of the temperodynamic structure of our data, and SD of the continuous time series (SDCONT) to obtain a measure of the distributional width of the signal.

We hypothesized that infants with lower OXTR methylation (increased gene expression in human cortex [46], presumed increased sensitivity to endogenous oxytocin) would show increased brain signal variability during social perception and would also receive more positive behavioral ratings. To test this hypothesis, we used a multivariate approach to simultaneously model the entire data structure including our epigene (OXTR methylation), brain (MSEAUC, SDCONT), and behavior (IBQ-R) measures using PLS-PM. The results of this model can be seen in Fig. 5 and Table 4 (model 1). First, we ensured that MSEAUC and SDCONT were identified as unique, distinguishable constructs in the model by confirming that the loadings for each electrode exceeded .5 [71] for its own construct (MSEAUC: M = .70, SDCONT: M = .67) and that the cross-loadings for each electrode did not exceed .5 onto the other construct (MSEAUC: M < .01, SDCONT: M < .01). We found a significant negative curvilinear association between OXTR methylation and MSEAUC (β = − 0.26, SE = 0.12, p = .014, f2 = .07) such that infants with lower OXTR methylation showed increased brain signal entropy. We simultaneously found a significant positive curvilinear association between MSEAUC and IBQ-R (β = 0.35, SE = 0.19, p = .035, f2 = .10) such that infants that showed greater entropy during social perception received more positive behavioral ratings. However, we did not find any significant associations between SDCONT and OXTR methylation (β = 0.10, SE = 0.12, p = .197) or IBQ-R (β = − .07, SE = 0.17, p = .346). While all electrodes loaded significantly onto the MSEAUC construct in our model, we obtained significantly higher loading coefficients (t = 3.12, p = .006)—indicating strongest associations in the model—for frontal and temporal (FP1, FP2, F9, F7, F3, FZ, F4, F8, F10, FC5, FC6, T7, T8, TP9, TP10) electrodes (M = .74) compared to all other (C3, CZ, C4, CP5, CP6, P7, P3, PZ, P4, P8, O1, O2) electrodes (M = .65). A multi-group analysis revealed no significant differences in path coefficients across male and female participants (all two-tailed p values ≥ .753). Table 5 contains results for model 1 using alternate MSE and SD computation methods.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Brain signal entropy is associated with OXTR methylation and accounts for individual differences in infant behavior. a Results from the partial least squares path model (study 1, model 1, n = 55) showing associations between OXTR methylation (OXTRm), area under the multiscale entropy curve (MSEAUC) evoked during social perception, standard deviation (SDCONT) of the continuous time series evoked during social perception, and ratings on the Revised Infant Behavioral Questionnaire (IBQ-R). β, path model coefficient; p, jackknifed p value for coefficient. b Topographical map showing loadings of each electrode on the MSEAUC construct. c Plot of the significant association between MSEAUC and OXTRm standardized factor scores. d Plot of the significant association between MSEAUC and IBQ-R standardized factor scores

Table 5 Model 1 results using alternative brain signal variability computation methods

Evoked entropy during social perception is associated with infant social but not non-social behavior

Next, we tested the hypothesis that brain signal variability evoked during social perception would specifically account for individual differences in social, but not non-social, behaviors. To test this hypothesis, we classified items in the IBQ-R subscores into Social and Non-Social constructs (see Table 3). The results of this model can be seen in Fig. 6 and Table 4 (model 2). We found the significant negative curvilinear association between OXTR methylation and MSEAUC persisted (β = − 0.27, SE = 0.17, p = .012, f2 = .07), and no significant associations emerged for SDCONT (ps ≥ .196). As hypothesized, we found that the significant positive curvilinear association between MSEAUC and behavior persisted only for the Social IBQ-R construct (β = 0.27, SE = 0.13, p = .025, f2 = .08). The association between MSEAUC and the Non-Social IBQ-R construct was not significant (β = 0.19, SE = 0.18, p = .152). Significant social behavioral indicators suggest that infants with lower OXTR methylation and higher MSEAUC evoked during social perception vocalize, enjoy cuddling, and approach social situations more, show less fear in social situations, and soothe easier through social interaction. A multi-group analysis revealed no significant differences in path coefficients across male and female participants (all two-tailed p values ≥ .320). Table 6 contains results for model 2 using alternate MSE and SD computation methods.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Evoked entropy during social perception is associated with infant social but not non-social behavior. a Results from the partial least squares path model (study 1, model 2, n = 55) showing associations between OXTR methylation (OXTRm), area under the multiscale entropy curve (MSEAUC) evoked during social perception, standard deviation (SDCONT) of the continuous time series evoked during social perception, and ratings on the Social and Non-Social constructs of the Revised Infant Behavioral Questionnaire (IBQ-R). β, path model coefficient; p, jackknifed p value for coefficient. b Topographical map showing loadings of each electrode on the MSEAUC construct. c Plot of the significant association between MSEAUC and OXTRm standardized factor scores. d Plot of the significant association between MSEAUC and Social IBQ-R standardized factor scores

Table 6 Model 2 results using alternative brain signal variability computation methods

Ongoing entropy does not show social-behavioral specificity

Finally, we examined whether these associations between OXTR methylation, brain signal variability, and infant social behavior occurred specifically due to the fact that infants were engaged in social perception during brain signal measurement, or if spontaneous, ongoing brain signal variability is associated with infant behavior regardless of perceptual context. To assess ongoing neural variability, we randomly extracted segments of brain signal from the inter-stimulus interval that were not time-locked to and did not overlap with stimulus presentation and re-ran the previous model with brain signal variability calculated on this spontaneous, ongoing signal. We found evoked and ongoing MSEAUC are significantly correlated across all electrodes (rs range from .57 to .84, all ps < .001). We again found that the significant negative curvilinear association between OXTR methylation and MSEAUC persisted (β = − 0.33, SE = 0.13, p = .007, f2 = .11), whereas no significant associations emerged for SDCONT (ps ≥ .174). Interestingly, this analysis revealed a significant positive curvilinear association between MSEAUC and both Social (β = 0.26, SE = 0.12, p = .014, f2 = .08) and Non-Social (β = 0.36, SE = 0.19, p = .032, f2 = .11) IBQ-R constructs, suggesting spontaneous, ongoing entropy, outside of a perceptual context, is associated with general, non-context-specific infant behavior. Significant behavioral indicators demonstrate that infants who showed greater ongoing brain signal entropy soothe easier through both social and non-social means, are more likely to approach and show excitement for both social and non-social activities, show greater perceptual sensitivity to non-social stimuli, enjoy cuddling more, and show less fear in social situations. The results of this model can be seen in Fig. 7 and Table 4 (model 3). A multi-group analysis revealed no significant differences in path coefficients across male and female participants (all two-tailed p values ≥ .316). Table 7 contains results for model 3 using alternate MSE and SD computation methods.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Ongoing entropy does not show social-behavioral specificity. a Results from the partial least squares path model (study 1, model 3, n = 55) showing associations between OXTR methylation (OXTRm), ongoing area under the multiscale entropy curve (MSEAUC), ongoing signal standard deviation of the continuous time series (SDCONT), and ratings on the Social and Non-Social constructs of the Revised Infant Behavioral Questionnaire (IBQ-R). β, path model coefficient; p, jackknifed p-value for coefficient. b Topographical map showing loadings of each electrode on the MSEAUC construct. c Plot of the significant association between MSEAUC and OXTRm standardized factor scores. d Plot of the significant association between MSEAUC and Social IBQ-R standardized factor scores. e Plot of the significant association between MSEAUC and Non-Social IBQ-R standardized factor scores

Table 7 Model 3 results using alternative brain signal variability computation methods

Study 2

Entropy shows modality-specific associations with infant social behavior

In study 2, we consider modality-specific associations between OXTR methylation, brain signal entropy evoked by social relative to non-social stimuli, and infant verbal (vocalization) and visual (attention to faces) social behavior in 4-month-old infants. We find, as in study 1, a negative association between OXTR methylation and social auditory-evoked MSEAUC (β = − 0.25, SE = 0.12, p = .026, f2 = .06) and a positive association between social auditory-evoked MSEAUC and infant social (verbal) behavior (β = 0.19, SE = 0.10, p = .031, f2 = .04). However, social auditory-evoked MSEAUC is not associated with infant social visual behavior (β = − 0.09, p = .349). There is also no association between OXTR methylation and visually evoked MSEAUC (β = 0.14, p = .182), nor between visually evoked MSEAUC and visual (β = 0.07, p = .311) or verbal (β = 0.14, p = .408) social behavior at 4 months of age. Topographical loadings for auditory-evoked MSEAUC load strongest onto left temporal electrodes. The results of this model can be seen in Fig. 8 and Table 4 (study 2).

Fig. 8
figure 8

Entropy shows modality-specific associations with infant social behavior. a Results from the partial least squares path model (study 2, n = 60) showing associations between OXTR methylation (OXTRm), area under the multiscale entropy curve (MSEAUC) for auditory and visual modalities, and infant social verbal and visual behavior. β, path model coefficient; p, jackknifed p-value for coefficient. b Topographical map showing loadings of each electrode on the Auditory MSEAUC construct. c Plot of the significant association between Auditory MSEAUC and OXTRm standardized factor scores. d Plot of the significant association between Auditory MSEAUC and infant verbal behavior standardized factor scores

Multiscale entropy is a reliable measure in infancy

Finally, a subset of 4-month-old infants returned within 1 week of their study appointment to determine the test-retest reliability of brain signal entropy (n = 10). MSE was found to show good reliability within 1 week (ICC = .73, p = .004). MSE test-retest reliability curves are plotted in Fig. 9.

Fig. 9
figure 9

Multiscale entropy is a reliable measure in infancy. Average multiscale entropy curves for scales 1 to 50 (500 to 10 Hz) are plotted for ten study 2 infants who underwent EEG at 4 months of age (test visit, black), and repeated the procedure within 1 week (re-test visit, red). We find good reliability (ICC = .73, p = .004) across the 1-week timespan

Discussion

Using a multivariate, prediction-based model, we show for the first time associations between early-life OXTR methylation, brain signal entropy, and infant behavior across two independent studies. Specifically, infants with lower levels of OXTR methylation (and likely increased sensitivity to endogenous oxytocin) show increased MSE during social perception, which is associated with more positive ratings specific to social behaviors. Our study 1 results demonstrate that these associations are (1) measure-specific—entropy, but not signal variance, links OXTR methylation and infant behavior, and (2) context-sensitive—entropy evoked during social perception specifically explains social behavior only. Our study 2 results replicate the association between OXTR methylation, social auditory-evoked MSE, and infant social behavior and suggest a modality-specific effect in infancy. Specifically, at 4 months of age, we only obtained significant epigene-brain-behavior associations within the auditory modality.

The current findings critically extend a growing body of research highlighting MSE as a powerful indicator of behavioral and developmental outcomes [14, 98, 99] and establish the reliability of this measure in infancy for the first time. In our analyses, we compared the explanatory power of two measures of brain signal variability and found that only MSE shows significant links between OXTR methylation and infant behavior. SD, a measure of overall distributional width, has been an effective measure for identifying group-level differences between healthy and clinical populations [54,55,56] or young and old adults [66, 74]. However, we may have found significant results with MSE and not SD in our sample of healthy infants because MSE is sensitive to temporal dependencies in a time series (Fig. 2) and is measured across multiple time scales. These distinctions may enable MSE to index neurodevelopmental changes that occur very early in life or are indicative of individual differences within even the healthy range of the continuum of human social behavior.

In addition to understanding what measures of neural variability are capable of explaining developmental or behavioral differences, it is also important to understand when neural variability is exploited to benefit perception or behavior. Neural activity can be categorized into two primary states: spontaneous ongoing brain activity, considered the default or “resting” state of the brain, and evoked brain activity that occurs in response to specific stimulation. It is thought that spontaneous, ongoing variability predominates in the brain, and evoked variability represents a relatively small proportion of overall variability that operates on top of ongoing variability to enable relevant behaviors [3], perhaps by optimizing sensory encoding and enhancing subsequent representations [10]. Our MSE study 1 results are in line with this understanding of brain variability. Infants that show greater ongoing MSE receive more positive behavioral ratings across contexts, perhaps reflecting a more dynamic, mature neural system [21, 52, 100] in general (trait) among these infants. However, infants that show higher MSE during social perception (state) might show enhanced social behaviors because they are able to build better perceptual and cognitive representations of complex social stimuli specifically, enabling particular sensitivity and flexibility to social stimuli. Spontaneous and evoked activity are understood to be intricately linked [19, 31, 101,102,103], and indeed, we find evoked and ongoing MSE are significantly correlated. These findings corroborate other research [10] showing that brain signal entropy reflects both trait-like differences across individuals and state-like variability within an individual.

Results of study 2 suggest a modality-specific association between brain signal variability and infant social behavior. Specifically, we find 4-month-old infants that show increased entropy to social, relative to non-social stimuli within the auditory modality vocalize more frequently. However, brain signal entropy evoked during visual perception was not associated with their verbal behavior. Neither entropy within the visual nor the auditory modality was associated with the infant’s visual attention to social stimuli at 4 months of age. These results replicate and extend study 1 by including a younger infant sample, measuring OXTR methylation, EEG, and behavior at the same timepoint, and introducing both visual and auditory stimuli across social and non-social domains. Together, these findings highlight the importance of early-life social auditory perception for the developing infant. Converging lines of research suggest that young infants predominately rely on auditory cues for social perception [104]. For example, 5-month-old infants consistently respond differentially to positive and negative voices but not faces [105], suggesting infants are more sensitive to voices than faces in early infancy. Furthermore, in a social referencing paradigm in which infants use the emotional expression of their mother to regulate their own behavior, mother’s vocalized fear alone, but not fearful face alone, towards an object was sufficient for 12-month-old infants to avoid the object [106].

This auditory dominance in infancy is unsurprising. As with many mammals, the auditory system develops much earlier than the visual system [107], and in humans, visual acuity does not reach adult levels until the third year of life [108]. Our modality-specific results support the hypothesis that neural variability is exploited to benefit perception or behavior from early in development. It is possible that young infants show higher brain signal entropy during social auditory perception because of enhanced perceptual experience with that stimulus class, enabling more robust cognitive representations and more effective production of vocalizations.

Our results have important implications for our understanding of neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism. We find the highest loadings for socially evoked MSE over frontal and temporal electrodes (Figs. 5b, 6b, 8b), indicating that irregularity in these regions most accounts for our epigene-brain-behavior associations. These regions are directly implicated in the oxytocinergic signaling pathway [109] and are critical for supporting social-cognitive processes [110] that emerge early in infancy [24]. Individuals with autism—a neurodevelopmental disorder marked by social impairment—show atypical neural development, particularly in frontal and temporal lobes [111]. These differences are thought to be reflected in altered brain signal entropy that occurs in autism [98, 112] even before diagnostic behaviors emerge [59, 99]. Differences within the oxytocinergic system are also implicated in autism, including increased OXTR methylation in both the brain and blood at site -934 [46]. Here we present data from two independent infant samples that may provide the foundation for a unifying, mechanistic account of social neurodevelopment, showing that early-life epigenetic differences in OXTR are associated with brain signal entropy during social perception to explain individual differences in social behavior in a context- and modality-specific manner.

Limitations and future directions

Our approach specifically targeted a single epigenetic site because of its association with gene expression in human cortex [46], variability across the general population [47], and sensitivity to early-life experiences in both causal (neural) and peripheral tissue [46, 50]. However, complex social behavioral phenotypes do not arise from the actions of a single molecular system. While it is remarkable that DNA methylation of a single OXTR CpG site significantly explains neural MSE, we acknowledge that the effect sizes are moderate and the overall variance explained by our models is fairly low. These results may be strengthened through a more comprehensive examination of markers of individual differences in oxytocinergic system function. Other molecular systems capable of impacting neural variability have been identified in human and animal models, such as the vasopressin [30] and dopaminergic [113] systems. Future longitudinal studies should simultaneously assay brain signal variability while targeting multiple genetic and molecular systems to gain a more complete understanding of the factors impacting neural variability in development.

Critically, we established here that OXTR methylation at site -934 is highly conserved in blood and saliva in a secondary, adult population, and we have previously shown that methylation at this site does not vary across cell types [48]. Demonstrating these associations is important because methylation plays a role in cell-type differentiation which may cause methylation patterns to vary across tissues. However, these results warrant replication in a developmental sample because the cellular composition of tissues may also vary with age [114].

Given the association between signal variability, signal detection [2, 3], and sensory encoding [10], we hypothesize that oxytocin ultimately regulates social behavior by increasing perceptual and attentional biases [48] to social information that drive experience-dependent neural development and are reflected in increased brain signal entropy during social perception. One limitation of the present studies is that we primarily relied on a parent-report measure to examine individual differences in infant behavior, although it should be noted that this measure has been validated by showing good correspondence with direct behavioral measures [36,37,38]. Nonetheless, future studies should include additional overt measures of infant perception and attention to more directly test the proposed attentional mechanism underlying the epigene-brain-behavior associations observed in the current studies.

Here, we add to important work to establish the reliability and validity of MSE [63, 115, 116] and to suggest its validity as a measure of individual differences [116]. However, a limitation to these and the majority of other studies investigating EEG variability metrics to date is the focus on a limited number of manually selected metrics with minimal consideration of alternatives [117]. A comprehensive examination of how recording, preprocessing, and time series algorithm choices influence the ability to detect associations with individual difference factors, be it age, disease state, or behavioral outcomes, is needed.

It is also important to acknowledge that future confirmatory research with larger samples is needed to more directly assess the theorized associations put forth by our models. Nonetheless, our use of predictive modeling capable of accounting for curvilinear associations between variables is a strength of the current studies. Few biological associations are likely linear in nature. Instead, many phenomena show u- or inverted u-shaped functions in which extremes on either end of the spectrum diverge from an optimal level [75,76,77]. Including high-risk populations to study normative and atypical development concurrently will be critical in future work to identify how neurobiological processes influence pathways of development towards or away from positive outcomes. Furthermore, both the quantity and the timing of oxytocin-regulated brain signal entropy likely impacts social development, and too much variability too soon in development, or too little too late, may result in suboptimal outcomes. Future research employing a longitudinal approach will therefore be critical for understanding how one’s genes, brain, and behavior interact throughout development to create a uniquely social individual.

Conclusions

The first year of life constitutes a time of rapid and dramatic changes in behavioral repertoire, cognitive ability, and neural architecture. During this time, developing infants are confronted with the daunting task of making sense of the world as they are bombarded with competing, fluctuating, and often ambiguous external stimuli. Understanding how the brain develops to form accurate models of the external world and generates appropriate behavioral responses is a significant and critical question of widespread multidisciplinary interest. Social information is particularly complex and dynamic, and the ability to perceive, interpret, elicit, and respond to social information is critical for an infant’s ability to survive and learn [118]. Our results suggest a mechanism by which early-life individual differences in the endogenous oxytocinergic system may drive unique neurodevelopmental trajectories affecting social behavior. These results hold implications for identifying individuals at risk for atypical development before behavioral manifestations of disorder occur and suggest potential biomarkers with probable diagnostic, therapeutic, and prognostic value.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Change history

  • 12 September 2020

    An amendment to this paper has been published and can be accessed via the original article.

Abbreviations

AOIs:

Areas of interest

AUC:

Area under the curve

AVE:

Average variance extracted

CBSEM:

Covariance-based structural equation modeling

CpG:

Cytosine-phosphate-guanine

DNA:

Deoxyribonucleic acid

EEG:

Electroencephalography

EOG:

Electrooculogram

ERP:

Event-related potential

fMRI:

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

GFP:

Global field power

IBQ-R:

Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire

IBQ-RS:

Short Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire

ICA:

Independent components analysis

ICC:

Interclass correlation coefficient

MSE:

Multiscale entropy

MSE1 :

Multiscale entropy of scale 1

MSE50 :

Multiscale entropy of scale 50

MSE100 :

Multiscale entropy of scale 100

MSEAUC :

Area under the multiscale entropy curve

MSESDRes :

Multiscale entropy with standard deviation residualized

OXTR :

Oxytocin receptor gene

PBMC:

Peripheral blood mononuclear cell

PLS-PM:

Partial least squares path modeling

SD:

Standard deviation

SD1 :

Standard deviation of scale 1

SD50 :

Standard deviation of scale 50

SD100 :

Standard deviation of scale 100

SDAUC :

Area under the course-grained standard deviation curve

SDCONT :

Standard deviation of the continuous time series

SDTXT :

Standard deviation across trials

SE:

Standard error

References

  1. Faisal AA, Selen LPJ, Wolpert DM. Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:292–303.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Ward LM, Doesburg SM, Kitajo K, MacLean SE, Roggeveen AB. Neural synchrony in stochastic resonance, attention, and consciousness. Can J Exp Psychol Can Psychol Exp. 2006;60:319.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Garrett DD, Samanez-Larkin GR, MacDonald SWS, Lindenberger U, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. Moment-to-moment brain signal variability: a next frontier in human brain mapping. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:610–24.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Stein RB, Gossen ER, Jones KE. Neuronal variability: noise or part of the signal. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:389–97.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Garrett DD, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. Moment-to-moment signal variability in the human brain can inform models of stochastic facilitation now. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:612.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Malins JG, Pugh KR, Buis B, Frost SJ, Hoeft F, Landi N, et al. Individual differences in reading skill are related to trial-by-trial neural activation variability in the reading network. J Neurosci. 2018;38:2981–9.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. McIntosh AR, Kovacevic N, Lippe S, Garrett D, Grady C, Jirsa V. The development of a noisy brain. Arch Ital Biol. 2010;148:323–37.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. McIntosh AR, Kovacevic N, Itier RJ. Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLoS Comput Biol. 2008;4:1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Mills T, Taylor MJ, McIntosh AR. Brain noise is task dependent and region specific. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104:2667–76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Waschke L, Wöstmann M, Obleser J. States and traits of neural irregularity in the age-varying human brain. Sci Rep. 2017;7:17381.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. The importance of being variable. J Neurosci. 2011;31:4496–503.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. De Wel O, Lavanga M, Caicedo Dorado A, Jansen K, Dereymaeker A, Naulaer G, et al. Complexity analysis of neonatal EEG using multiscale entropy: applications in brain maturation and sleep stage classification. Entropy. 2017;19:1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Hasegawa C, Takahashi T, Yoshimura Y, Nobukawa S, Ikeda T, Saito DN, et al. Developmental trajectory of infant brain signal variability: a longitudinal pilot study. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:566.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Miskovic V, Owens M, Kuntzelman K, Gibb BE. Charting moment-to-moment brain signal variability from early to late childhood. Cortex. 2016;83:51–61.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Lippé S, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR. Differential maturation of brain signal complexity in the human auditory and visual system. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;48:1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Nomi JS, Bolt TS, Ezie CEC, Uddin LQ, Heller AS. Moment-to-moment BOLD signal variability reflects regional changes in neural flexibility across the lifespan. J Neurosci. 2017;37:5539–48.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Polizzotto NR, Takahashi T, Walker CP, Cho RY. Wide range multiscale entropy changes through development. Entropy. 2016;18:12.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Kaffashi F, Scher MS, Ludington-Hoe SM, Loparo KA. An analysis of the kangaroo care intervention using neonatal EEG complexity: a preliminary study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124:238–46.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Shew WL, Yang H, Petermann T, Roy R, Plenz D. Neuronal avalanches imply maximum dynamic range in cortical networks at criticality. J Neurosci. 2009;29:15595–600.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Shew WL, Yang H, Yu S, Roy R, Plenz D. Information capacity and transmission are maximized in balanced cortical networks with neuronal avalanches. J Neurosci. 2011;31:55–63.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Fuchs E, Ayali A, Robinson A, Hulata E, Ben-Jacob E. Coemergence of regularity and complexity during neural network development. Dev Neurobiol. 2007;67:1802–14.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Mišić B, Doesburg SM, Fatima Z, Vidal J, Vakorin VA, Taylor MJ, et al. Coordinated information generation and mental flexibility: large-scale network disruption in children with autism. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25:2815–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Wang S, Adolphs R. Social Saliency. In: Zhao Q, editor. Computational and cognitive neuroscience of vision. Singapore: Springer; 2017. p. 171–93.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Grossmann T. The development of social brain functions in infancy. Psychol Bull. 2015;141:1266–87.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Carter CS, Grippo AJ, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Ruscio MG, Porges SW. Oxytocin, vasopressin and sociality. Prog Brain Res. 2008;170:331–6.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Abu-Akel A. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79:194–202.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Owen SF, Tuncdemir SN, Bader PL, Tirko NN, Fishell G, Tsien RW. Oxytocin enhances hippocampal spike transmission by modulating fast-spiking interneurons. Nature. 2013;500:458–62.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Marlin BJ, Mitre M, D’amour JA, Chao MV, Froemke RC. Oxytocin enables maternal behaviour by balancing cortical inhibition. Nature. 2015;520:499–504.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Maier P, Kaiser ME, Grinevich V, Draguhn A, Both M. Differential effects of oxytocin on mouse hippocampal oscillations in vitro. Eur J Neurosci. 2016;44:2885–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Mühlethaler M, Charpak S, Dreifuss JJ. Contrasting effects of neurohypophysial peptides on pyramidal and non-pyramidal neurones in the rat hippocampus. Brain Res. 1984;308:97–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Arviv O, Goldstein A, Shriki O. Near-critical dynamics in stimulus-evoked activity of the human brain and its relation to spontaneous resting-state activity. J Neurosci. 2015;35:13927–42.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Beggs JM, Plenz D. Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. J Neurosci. 2003;23:11167–77.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Poil S-S, Hardstone R, Mansvelder HD, Linkenkaer-Hansen K. Critical-state dynamics of avalanches and oscillations jointly emerge from balanced excitation/inhibition in neuronal networks. J Neurosci. 2012;3:9817–23.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Agrawal V, Cowley AB, Alfaori Q, Larremore DB, Restrepo JG, Shew WL. Robust entropy requires strong and balanced excitatory and inhibitory synapses. Chaos An Interdiscip J Nonlinear Sci. 2018;28:103115.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2003;26:64–86.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Gartstein MA, Marmion J. Fear and positive affectivity in infancy: convergence/discrepancy between parent-report and laboratory-based indicators. Infant Behav Dev. 2008;31:227–38.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Goldsmith HH, Campos JJ. The structure of temperamental fear and pleasure in infants: a psychometric perspective. Child Dev. 1990;61:1944–64.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Diaz A, Bell MA. Frontal EEG asymmetry and fear reactivity in different contexts at 10 months. Dev Psychobiol. 2012;54:536–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Erickson NL, Gartstein MA, Beauchaine TP. Infant predictors of toddler effortful control: a multi-method developmentally sensitive approach. Infant Child Dev. 2017;26:e1971.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Kozlova EA, Slobodskaya HR, Gartstein MA. Early temperament as a predictor of child mental health. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2019:1–14 https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s11469-019-00181-3.

  41. Gartstein MA, Putnam SP, Rothbart MK. Etiology of preschool behavior problems: contributions of temperament attributes in early childhood. Infant Ment Health J. 2012;33:197–211.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Slobodskaya HR, Kozlova EA. Early temperament as a predictor of later personality. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;99:127–32.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Putnam SP, Helbig AL, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK, Leerkes E. Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire–Revised. J Pers Assess. 2014;96:445–58.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. MacLean EL, Wilson SR, Martin WL, Davis JM, Nazarloo HP, Carter CS. Challenges for measuring oxytocin: the blind men and the elephant? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019;107:225–31.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Connelly JJ, Morris JP. Epigenetics and social behavior. In: Chiao JY, Li S-C, Seligman R, Turner R, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Cultural Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press; 2016. p. 299–313.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Gregory SG, Connelly JJ, Towers AJ, Johnson J, Biscocho D, Markunas CA, et al. Genomic and epigenetic evidence for oxytocin receptor deficiency in autism. BMC Med. 2009;7:62.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Puglia MH, Lillard TS, Morris JP, Connelly JJ. Epigenetic modification of the oxytocin receptor gene influences the perception of anger and fear in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112:3308–13.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Puglia MH, Connelly JJ, Morris JP. Epigenetic regulation of the oxytocin receptor is associated with neural response during selective social attention. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8:116.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Jack A, Connelly JJ, Morris JP, James P. DNA methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene predicts neural response to ambiguous social stimuli. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:280.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Perkeybile AM, Carter CS, Wroblewski KL, Puglia MH, Kenkel WM, Lillard TS, et al. Early nurture epigenetically tunes the oxytocin receptor. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018;99:128–36.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Costa M, Goldberger AL, Peng C-K. Multiscale entropy analysis of complex physiologic time series. Phys Rev Lett. 2002;89:068102.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Courtiol J, Perdikis D, Petkoski S, Müller V, Huys R, Sleimen-Malkoun R, et al. The multiscale entropy: Guidelines for use and interpretation in brain signal analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2016;273:175–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Vakorin VA, Lippe S, McIntosh AR. Variability of brain signals processed locally transforms into higher connectivity with brain development. J Neurosci. 2011;31:6405–13.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Dinstein I, Heeger DJ, Lorenzi L, Minshew NJ, Malach R, Behrmann M. Unreliable evoked responses in autism. Neuron. 2012;75:981–91.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Milne E. Increased intra-participant variability in children with autistic spectrum disorders: evidence from single-trial analysis of evoked EEG. Front Psychol. 2011;2:51.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Haigh SM, Heeger DJ, Dinstein I, Minshew N, Behrmann M. Cortical variability in the sensory-evoked response in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:1176–90.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Bosl W, Tierney A, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson C. EEG complexity as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorder risk. BMC Med. 2011;9:18.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Nagy C, Turecki G. Sensitive periods in epigenetics: bringing us closer to complex behavioral phenotypes. Epigenomics. 2012;4:445–57.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Cao M, Huang H, He Y. Developmental connectomics from infancy through early childhood. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40:494–506.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Missana M, Altvater-Mackensen N, Grossmann T. Neural correlates of infants’ sensitivity to vocal expressions of peers. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;26:39–44.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134:9–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Stets M, Stahl D, Reid VM. A meta-analysis investigating factors underlying attrition rates in infant ERP studies. Dev Neuropsychol. 2012;37:226–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Grandy TH, Garrett DD, Schmiedek F, Werkle-Bergner M. On the estimation of brain signal entropy from sparse neuroimaging data. Sci Rep. 2016;6:23073.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Richman JS, Moorman JR. Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy. Am J Physiol Circ Physiol. 2000;278:H2039–49.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Nikulin VV, Brismar T. Comment on “Multiscale entropy analysis of complex physiologic time series”. Phys Rev Lett. 2004;92:89803.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. Blood oxygen level-dependent signal variability is more than just noise. J Neurosci. 2010;30:4914–21.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Shafiei G, Zeighami Y, Clark CA, Coull JT, Nagano-Saito A, Leyton M, et al. Dopamine signaling modulates the stability and integration of intrinsic brain networks. Cereb Cortex. 2019;29:397–409.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Revelle WR. psych: procedures for personality and psychological research; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  69. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Field A, Miles J, Field Z. Discovering statistics using R. London: Sage Publications; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Esposito Vinzi V, Chin WW, Henseler J, Wang H. editors. Handbook of partial least squares. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Ravand H. Partial least squares structural equation modeling with R. Pract Assessment, Res Eval. 2016;21:1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  73. McIntosh AR, Lobaugh NJ. Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: applications and advances. Neuroimage. 2004;23(SUPPL. 1):S250–63.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Grady CL, Garrett DD. Understanding variability in the BOLD signal and why it matters for aging. Brain Imaging Behav. 2014;8:274–83.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Gershkoff-Stowe L, Thelen E. U-shaped changes in behavior: a dynamic systems perspective. J Cogn Dev. 2009;5:11–36.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Diamond DM. Cognitive, endocrine and mechanistic perspectives on non-linear relationships between arousal and brain function. Nonlinearity Biol Toxicol Med. 2005;3:1.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Northoff G, Tumati S. “Average is good, extremes are bad” – non-linear inverted U-shaped relationship between neural mechanisms and functionality of mental features. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;104:11–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Kock N. WarpPLS; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Kock N. Using WarpPLS in e-collaboration studies: an overview of five main analysis steps. Int J e-Collaboration. 2010;6:1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Bagozzi RP, Yi Y. On the evaluation of structural equation models. J Acad Mark Sci. 1988;16:74–94.

    Google Scholar 

  81. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Kock N. Advanced mediating effects tests, multi-group analyses, and measurement model assessments in PLS-based SEM. Int J e-Collaboration. 2014;10:1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Chin WW, Newsted PR. Structural equation modeling analysis with small samples using partial least squares. In: Hoyle R, editor. Statistical strategies for small sample research. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1999. p. 307–41.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41:1149–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Bujang MA, Baharum N. A simplified guide to determination of sample size requirements for estimating the value of intraclass correlation coefficient: a review. Arch Orofac Sci. 2017;12:1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  86. van der Schalk J, Hawk ST, Fischer AH, Doosje B. Moving faces, looking places: validation of the Amsterdam Dynamic Facial Expression Set (ADFES). Emotion. 2011;11:907–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Geusebroek J-M, Burghouts GJ, Smeulders AWM. The Amsterdam Library of Object Images. Int J Comput Vis. 2005;61:103–12.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Willenbockel V, Sadr J, Fiset D, Horne GO, Gosselin F, Tanaka JW. Controlling low-level image properties: the SHINE toolbox. Behav Res Methods. 2010;42:671–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Brent MR, Siskind JM. The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition. 2001;81:B33–44.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. MacWhinney B. The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk. 3rd ed. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Boersma P, Weenink D. Praat: doing phonetics by computer; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10:443–6.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Gamer M, Lemon, Singh J, Fellows Puspendra Singh I. irr: Various Coefficients of Interrater Reliability and Agreement. 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  94. Koo TK, Li MY. A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15:155–63.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Chevallier C, Parish-Morris J, McVey A, Rump KM, Sasson NJ, Herrington JD, et al. Measuring social attention and motivation in autism spectrum disorder using eye-tracking: stimulus type matters. Autism Res. 2015;8:620–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. The OA, Tobii I-VT. Fixation filter algorithm description. 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  97. Leppänen JM, Forssman L, Kaatiala J, Yrttiaho S, Wass S. Widely applicable MATLAB routines for automated analysis of saccadic reaction times. Behav Res Methods. 2015;47:538.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Catarino A, Churches O, Baron-Cohen S, Andrade A, Ring H. Atypical EEG complexity in autism spectrum conditions: a multiscale entropy analysis. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122:2375–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Bosl WJ, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. EEG analytics for early detection of autism spectrum disorder: a data-driven approach. Sci Rep. 2018;8:6828.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Heisz JJ, Shedden JM, McIntosh AR. Relating brain signal variability to knowledge representation. Neuroimage. 2012;63:1384–92.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Arieli A, Sterkin A, Grinvald A, Aertsen A. Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science. 1996;273:1868–71.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Tsodyks M, Kenet T, Grinvald A, Arieli A. Linking spontaneous activity of single cortical neurons and the underlying functional architecture. Science. 1999;286:1943–6.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Bolt T, Anderson ML, Uddin LQ. Beyond the evoked/intrinsic neural process dichotomy. Netw Neurosci. 2018;2:1–22.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  104. Walker-Andrews AS. Infants’ perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. Psychol Bull. 1997;121:437–56.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Fernald A. Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: an evolutionary perspective. In: Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J, editors. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press; 1992. p. 391–428.

    Google Scholar 

  106. Mumme DL, Fernald A, Herrera C. Infants’ responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Dev. 1996;67:3219–37.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Gottlieb G. Ontogenesis of sensory function in birds and mammals. In: Tobach E, Aronson LR, Shaw E, editors. The biopsychology of development. New York: Academic Press; 1971. p. 67–128.

    Google Scholar 

  108. Catford GV, Oliver A. Development of visual acuity. Arch Dis Child. 1973;48:47–50.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:524–38.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Adolphs R. The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:693–716.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. Hoppenbrouwers M, Vandermosten M, Boets B. Autism as a disconnection syndrome: a qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2014;8:387–412.

    Google Scholar 

  112. Chu Y-J, Chang C-F, Shieh J-S, Lee W-T, Chu Y-J, Chang C-F, et al. The potential application of multiscale entropy analysis of electroencephalography in children with neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders. Entropy. 2017;19:428.

    Google Scholar 

  113. Guitart-Masip M, Salami A, Garrett D, Rieckmann A, Lindenberger U, Bäckman L. BOLD variability is related to dopaminergic neurotransmission and cognitive aging. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26:2074–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Jaffe AE, Irizarry RA. Accounting for cellular heterogeneity is critical in epigenome-wide association studies. Genome Biol. 2014;15:R31.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  115. Kuntzelman K, Jack Rhodes L, Harrington LN, Miskovic V. A practical comparison of algorithms for the measurement of multiscale entropy in neural time series data. Brain Cogn. 2018;123:126–35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Kaur Y, Ouyang G, Junge M, Sommer W, Liu MM, Zhou C, et al. The reliability and psychometric structure of multi-scale entropy measured from EEG signals at rest and during face and object recognition tasks. J Neurosci Methods. 2019;326:108343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Fulcher BD, Little MA, Jones NS. Highly comparative time-series analysis: the empirical structure of time series and their methods. J R Soc Interface. 2013;10:20130048.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Nelson CA III, Furtado EA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH Jr. The deprived human brain. Am Sci. 2009;97:222.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the parents and infants who took part in this study; Diogo Fortes, Andrew Graves, and Amalia McDonald for help with Tissue Comparison Study data collection; Caterina Böttcher, Doris Goedikmeier, and Jenny Tippmann for help with study 1 infant data collection; Laura Atalla, Rachel Corney, Christiana King, and Rebecca Stafford for help with study 2 infant data collection; and Markus Werkle-Bergner for providing scripts to compute MSE across discontinuous segments.

Funding

This research was supported by funds from the Max Planck Society to T.G.; N.S.F. grant #1729289 to T.G., J.J.C., and J.P.M.; funds from the National Institute of Child Health and Development (F31HD090865), the American Psychological Foundation, the University of Virginia Brain Institute, and a PEO Scholar Award to M.H.P.; and a Hartwell Postdoctoral Fellowship to K.M.K.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M.H.P., J.P.M., J.J.C., and T.G. conceptualized and designed the study. K.M.K., M.M., and T.G. acquired infant EEG and behavioral data for study 1. K.M.K. collected and isolated infant salivary DNA for study 1. T.S.L. performed infant epigenetic analysis for study 1. M.H.P. and C.L.W. acquired infant data for study 2. M.H.P. analyzed the data, conducted the Tissue Comparison Study, performed the infant epigenetic analysis for study 2, and wrote the manuscript. C.L.W., K.M.K, M.M., J.P.M., J.J.C., and T.G. provided comments and edits to the manuscript. M.H.P., K.M.K., J.P.M., J.J.C., and T.G. provided funding for the work. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Meghan H. Puglia.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All parents provided written informed consent prior to study 1, approved by the Leipzig University Ethics Committee, and the Tissue Comparison Study and Study 2, approved by the University of Virginia Health and Human Sciences Institutional Review Board.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The original version of this article was revised to correct Figures 1 and 6 which had inadvertently published online without the author’s final corrections being incorporated.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1.

Criterion latent variable quartile plots for all significant effects for each model.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Puglia, M.H., Krol, K.M., Missana, M. et al. Epigenetic tuning of brain signal entropy in emergent human social behavior. BMC Med 18, 244 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12916-020-01683-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12916-020-01683-x

Keywords