Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Lower gestational age is associated with lower cortical volume and cognitive and educational performance in adolescence

Abstract

Background

Gestational age (GA) is associated with later cognition and behavior. However, it is unclear how specific cognitive domains and brain structural development varies with the stepwise change of gestational duration.

Methods

This large-scale longitudinal cohort study analyzed 11,878 early adolescents’ brain volume maps at 9–10 years (baseline) and 5685 at 11–12 years (a 2-year follow-up) from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. According to gestational age, adolescents were divided into five categorical groups: ≤ 33 weeks, 34–35 weeks, 36 weeks, 37–39 weeks, and ≥ 40 weeks. The NIH Toolbox was used to estimate neurocognitive performance, including crystallized and fluid intelligence, which was measured for 11,878 adolescents at baseline with crystallized intelligence and relevant subscales obtained at 2-year follow-up (with participant numbers ranging from 6185 to 6310 depending on the cognitive domain). An additional large population-based cohort of 618,070 middle adolescents at ninth-grade (15–16 years) from the Danish national register was utilized to validate the association between gestational age and academic achievements. A linear mixed model was used to examine the group differences between gestational age and neurocognitive performance, school achievements, and grey matter volume. A mediation analysis was performed to examine whether brain structural volumes mediated the association between GA and neurocognition, followed with a longitudinal analysis to track the changes.

Results

Significant group differences were found in all neurocognitive scores, school achievements, and twenty-five cortical regional volumes (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Specifically, lower gestational ages were associated with graded lower cognition and school achievements and with smaller brain volumes of the fronto-parieto-temporal, fusiform, cingulate, insula, postcentral, hippocampal, thalamic, and pallidal regions. These lower brain volumes mediated the association between gestational age and cognitive function (P = 1 × 10−8, β = 0.017, 95% CI: 0.007–0.028). Longitudinal analysis showed that compared to full term adolescents, preterm adolescents still had smaller brain volumes and crystallized intelligence scores at 11–12 years.

Conclusions

These results emphasize the relationships between gestational age at birth and adolescents’ lower brain volume, and lower cognitive and educational performance, measured many years later when 9–10 and 11–12 years old. The study indicates the importance of early screening and close follow-up for neurocognitive and behavioral development for children and adolescents born with gestational ages that are even a little lower than full term.

Peer Review reports

Background

The prenatal period is a foundationally and universally critical phase for human brain development [1]. Thus, understanding how prenatal influences play out in later life has important implications for developmental neuroscience and for public health.

Brain volume and its developmental changes are related to cognitive functions [2]. The human brain shows an initial accelerating increase in gray matter volume from mid-gestation onwards, peaking around 6 years, and then slowing down and then shrinking roughly from late childhood into adulthood [2, 3]. During the third trimester, the human cerebral cortex undergoes especially rapid volumetric increase where there is marked growth with a fourfold increase and the emergence of sulci and gyri that is essential for human intelligence [4, 5]. Non-optimal development during these periods due to preterm birth may be associated with altered brain structure and function that persist during postnatal life [6,7,8] and higher morbidity and mortality [9, 10]. Therefore, understanding to what extent and how different gestational durations are associated with neurodevelopment in later life are essential to clarify our understanding of the association between prenatal factors and health outcomes.

Gestational age (GA) as a global proxy measure of in-utero progress of fetal development is closely related to health outcomes in children and adolescents [11, 12]. A substantial body of population-based evidence has shown that younger gestational age is associated with poorer cognitive and academic performance in children and adolescents [13,14,15]. Some neuroimaging studies indicated that gray matter volume or related structural measures (such as brain gyrification) may partially mediate the association between gestational age and cognitive function in children or adults [16,17,18]. Additionally, there are some longitudinal studies tracking the development of children’s and adolescents’ brain structures born with different gestational ages, with some evidence for “no catch-up” growth of brain volume in very preterm births compared to full-term births [19,20,21].

However, although not in all cases [17, 22], our current understanding of the relationship between GA, cognition and brain volume in adolescents largely stems from pairwise comparisons [21, 23, 24], such as comparing healthy controls born at full-term with those born very early preterm (< 26 gestational weeks) or very preterm (26 to 32 gestational weeks) or late preterm (34 to 36 gestational weeks). A definition of preterm birth as a dichotomous measure based on an arbitrary cut-off, rather than birth time at a specific fetal maturity level or at continuously graded gestational stages, limits our understanding of postnatal maturation. Most of the evidence on underlying brain structures mediating the association of prematurity and cognition is separate from studies focusing on preterm-related brain structural growth [17, 18, 21], and large-scale studies are needed that track the longitudinal development of specific brain structures linked to gestational age and cognition. Additionally, most previous investigations are limited to single-dimensional neurocognitive evaluation (e.g., IQ [21] or executive function [17]) or a small sample size [16, 19]. The current study focused on multi-dimensional cognitive characteristics including reading and language, working memory and attention, and processing speed. Moreover, gestational age may be associated with other confounding factors, such as birth weight and early Cesarean delivery due to unavoidable clinical complications, which may also affect the development of the offspring. This shows that there is a need for large-scale studies taking into account potential confounding variables to measure many aspects of brain structure, and of cognitive development into adolescence, and to relate these to continuously graded gestational age.

Given this background, the current investigation focused on clarifying the association between gestational age, the volumes of different brain regions, and dimensional measures of cognitive functions, using a large sample of 11,878 early adolescents aged 9–10 years with 2-year follow-up data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study [25] and validated by Danish cohort study of 618,070 adolescents at 15–16 years old [26]. The specific aims were as follows: (1) to assess whether shorter gestational duration is associated with a higher risk of poor cognitive function and lower volumes of some brain regions; (2) to examine whether the volume of some brain regions mediates the association between gestational age and cognition; (3) to examine whether different gestational ages have divergent growth trajectories for brain volumes and whether the association between gestational age and brain volume is still present in adolescence. It was hypothesized that shorter gestational duration is associated with poorer cognitive and academic performance in adolescents and that the underlying mechanisms could be related to long-lasting differences in brain structure in at least the temporal region because this is the latest brain area to mature during the third trimester.

Role of the funding source

The funding sources had no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Methods

Participants

The neuroimaging data and cognitive assessments used in this study were obtained from the Annual Curated Data Release 3.0 of the ABCD study (https://abcdstudy.org/scientists/data-sharing/), which is a large national-based longitudinal investigation of Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development across 21 research sites in the USA [27]. Participants and their parents or caregivers completed a set of visits consisting clinical interviews, surveys, neurocognitive tests, and neuroimaging. A visit for the population is ongoing every year for behavioral and cognitive assessments and every 2 years for neuroimaging scanning. In the current study, a total of 11,878 participants aged between 9 and 10 years were recruited at baseline. For follow-up data, we only included neuroimages, not cognitive performance which were largely missing. Participants with missing information of gestational age or blank cognitive assessments or whose magnetic resonance imaging scans did not pass quality control were excluded. A participant selection flowchart and demographics of the final enrolled participants in the neuroimaging analysis are provided in Fig. 1 and Table 1 respectively. The ABCD investigators obtained written and oral informed consent from parents and children, respectively [28]. More details of the subjects and the collection are provided at the ABCD website (https://abcdstudy.org/scientists/protocols/) and elsewhere [27, 29].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of the ABCD participants enrolled for neuroimaging analysis

Gestational age determination

Gestational age at birth was calculated based on the parent retrospective report on the following items: “Was the child born prematurely?” (devhx_12a_p) and “About how many weeks premature was the child when they were born?” (devhx_12_p). Considering that term adolescents defined in the second item include all adolescents born at full term defined in the first item and that the second item gives the specific number of premature weeks, we relied primarily on the second item to determine weeks of gestation. According to the answers, gestational age for term adolescents was defined as 40 weeks and preterm adolescents as 40 weeks minus the number of weeks premature. Participants were excluded if information was not provided (answering “Don't know’). The final gestational age ranges from less than 28 to 40 weeks. Referring to the grouping of gestational age in some previous research studies [12, 30, 31] and considering approximate matching of sample sizes, five exclusive groups were formed: adolescents born at less than 33 weeks, 34–35 weeks, 36 weeks, 37–39 weeks, and 40 weeks of gestation. We additionally compared the group differences in cognitive performance between each pair of gestational ages. The distribution of gestational age for neuroimaging analyses is provided in Fig. S1. Further, considering that grouping together < 28–33 weeks may not reflect the cognitive differences in this gestational age range, we divided this range of gestational age into ≤ 28 weeks and 29 to 33 weeks for validation.

Cognitive assessment

Cognitive performance was measured using the fully-corrected composite T score within the NIH Toolbox which is a standardized battery of cognitive tests to estimate general intellectual functioning [32]. It incorporates performance from seven different tests to characterize two kinds of cognition: crystallized and fluid intelligence. The crystallized intelligence score summarizes scores from (1) the oral reading recognition test and (2) the picture vocabulary test. The fluid intelligence score summarizes scores from (3) the flanker inhibitory control and attention test, (4) the list-sorting working memory test, (5) the dimensional change card sort test, (6) the pattern comparison processing speed test, and (7) the picture sequence memory test. We also included three other kinds of cognitive measures for validation, including the Little Man Task for measuring visuospatial processing flexibility and attention, the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test for verbal learning and memory, and the Matrix Reasoning Task for measuring fluid intelligence and visuospatial reasoning.

Neuroimaging

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanning was optimized and harmonized across the ABCD sites for 3-T scanners and preprocessing procedures were unified according to a pipeline performed by the ABCD team [33]. Cortical surface reconstruction and subcortical segmentation were processed through FreeSurfer v.5.3.0 on acquired T1-weighted anatomical scans by the ABCD team. The current study focused on brain cortical gray matter volume estimates using the Desikan-Killany Atlas [34] (34 regions per hemisphere) and subcortical volume estimates using the Automatic Subcortical Segmentation [35] (40 labels for ASEG regions). Image quality control was performed by the ABCD official team, and only the data recommended for use were included in our analysis. Detailed image preprocessing and quality control are shown in the Additional file 1 [27, 29, 33].

Statistical analysis

Cross-sectional analysis

A linear mixed-effects model (LMM) was used to test the group difference of gestational age on the volumes of brain regions and cognitive performance, implemented using the MATLAB function fitlme. Dependent variables in the model included regional volumes and cognitive scores. The independent variable was the five gestational age groups. A sets of nuisance covariates to be regressed out were modeled as fixed effects, and the imaging site was accounted for random intercept parameters. The following were the covariates of no interest: adolescents’ age, sex, body mass index, family income, parental education year, race (coded as three-column dummy variables: white, black and American Indian), puberty scores, maternal age at delivery, and prenatal exposure to any kind of substance (tobacco, marijuana, cocaine/crack, heroin/morphine, oxycontin) before or after maternal knowledge of pregnancy. An F-statistic was computed using MATLAB’s anova function to determine the difference between the gestational age groups. Then, a specified comparison was carried out between each preterm GA group and the 40-week full-term group to test how brain volume and cognitive performance varied with differences in the number of gestational weeks. A t-statistic was obtained in this step by using fitlme with 40 weeks serving as the reference level. Test of significance for both F- and t-statistics were corrected for multiple comparison using Bonferroni correction at the 0.05 level. Partial eta-square η2p and Cohen’s d values, representing the effect size of the group difference, were computed from the F statistic and t statistic respectively. The analysis was performed at both baseline (11,203 participants) and 2-year follow-up (5553 participants) for neuroimaging data and at baseline (11,847 participants) for all cognitive dimensions and at 2-year follow-up (number of participants ranging from 6185 to 6310) for some available cognitive domains.

Mediation analysis

This is to evaluate whether the covariance between gestational age and cognitive performance can be explained by the lower brain volumes identified for lower gestational ages. This procedure was performed using the Mediation Toolbox developed by Tor Wager’s group (https://github.com/canlab/MediationToolbox), which has been widely used in neuroimaging studies [36,37,38]. In the analysis, the independent (predictor) variable was gestational age ranging from 27 to 40 weeks, and the dependent (predicted) variable was cognitive score (the total, crystallized or fluid intelligence score). The proposed mediator (in the indirect path) was the mean cortical volume for regions identified as having group difference at baseline (Fig. 3A). We also took each individual regional volume as mediator and performed the same analysis (Table S4). Covariates used in the models were identical with those in the “ Cross-sectional analysis” section. The significance of the mediation was estimated by the bias-corrected bootstrap approach (with 10,000 random samplings), which has been proved to be more robust to nonnormality and has better type I error control than the causal steps method and the Sobel test [39]. All variables in the mediation analysis were based on baseline neuroimaging and cognitive data (11,203 participants) and were normalized to z-scores before calculations.

Longitudinal analysis

For regional volumes showing significant group differences at baseline, their developmental changes were measured 2 years later to detect whether adolescents have different developmental trajectories due to different gestational weeks. The longitudinal analysis was performed by a linear mixed model using the function fitlme. The dependent variables were the cortical volumes of significant brain regions and their average value. Independent variables were gestational age with five categorical groups and time with two-levels (baseline and 2-years follow-up). A set of the same nuisance covariates were the same as the “ Cross-sectional analysis” section and modeled as fixed effects, and the site was designed as random effect. Partial eta-square η2p, representing the effect size of the group-by-time interaction, was computed from the F statistic, with significance set as P < 0.05 by Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. The analysis was performed for 5296 participants who have both baseline and follow-up neuroimaging data.

Confounding factors

Due to the fact that birth weight usually has a high correlation with gestational weeks [13], we excluded adolescents with extremely high or low birth weight relative to the expected gestational weeks and then reanalyzed the effect of gestational weeks on cortical volume. Both small and large for gestational age (SGA and LGA) were excluded, which was defined as birth weight more than two SDs below (above) the mean weight relative to that expected for sex and gestational age, according to the Canadian national dataset [40]. Then, because cesarean delivery is unspontaneous premature delivery often caused by clinical complications, which may have different underlying mechanisms from spontaneous prematurity [41], we removed these participants and then replicated the analysis in the population with spontaneous delivery only. Moreover, as socioeconomic factors are consistently considered to be closely related to the brain and cognitive development [42,43,44], we tested whether family income was related to the association between gestational age and cortical volume. The family income was classified as three levels, with low level of < $50,000, middle level of $50,000–100,000, and high level of > $100,000. Then, a group-by-income interaction analysis was conducted to detect any relation of the income difference to the association between gestational weeks and cortical volumes at baseline.

External validation analysis using the Danish nationwide cohort study

Different from cognitive ability tests, academic achievement requires the integration of multiple cognitive domains and has practical educational significance because it reflects learning abilities in real society [45]. To clarify whether gestational age has a long-term association with mid-adolescents’ school achievements, we utilized a population-based cohort including 618,070 adolescents at the age of 15–16 years from the Danish national register (detailed characteristics see Table S1) [26, 36, 46,47,48,49]. In the Danish cohort, information on gestational age was obtained from the Danish Medical Birth Resister. The length of gestation was estimated by ultrasonography examination, last menstrual period, or clinical examination, which has been frequently used to estimate gestational age in previous studies [50, 51]. Examination grades in Danish and Mathematics, which consisted of five profile areas including oral, reading comprehension, spelling, problem solving and mathematical skills, were analyzed. The grades were standardized as z scores for each gestational age group (22 to 33, 34 to 35, 36, 37 to 39, and ≥ 40 gestational weeks). A multiple linear regression model was used to estimate the difference of school grades between each GA group and the reference group (GA ≥ 40 week). More details are provided in Additional file 1.

Results

Participant characteristics

This study evaluated 11,878 adolescents (5366 [47.9%] female) at baseline (9–10 years), with 5685 (2615 [46.0%] female) having 2-year follow-up data. Imaging data from 545 and 119 individuals were excluded respectively for the two time points because of failure to pass quality control measures. The final participants for baseline imaging analysis were divided into five exclusive groups according to gestational age, with 432, 528, 563, 542, and 9138 adolescents born at less than 33 weeks, 34–35 weeks, 36 weeks, 37–39 weeks, and 40 weeks (full term) of gestation, respectively. The demographics and participant selection procedures are presented in Table 1 and Fig. 1.

Younger gestational age and lower cognitive function at 9–10 years and 2 years later

Overall, differences of cognitive performances were associated with gestational age (GA) (P < 0.05 to P < 0.001, Bonferroni corrected; Fig. 2). A positive association was observed between GA and the total intelligence score (regression coefficient [β] = 0.40/week; 95% CI, 0.30–0.49/week), crystallized intelligence score (β = 0.40/week; 95% CI, 0.30–0.49/week) as well as fluid intelligence score (β = 0.26/week; 95% CI, 0.16–0.36/week). For every 1-week longer gestational duration, the total intelligence score, crystallized intelligence score, and fluid intelligence score increased by 0.8%, 0.8%, and 0.6%, respectively. Adolescents born at ≤ 33 week had an 8.6% decrease in total intelligence score compared with those at ≥ 40 week and a decline of 8.0% and 5.7% for crystallized intelligence score and fluid intelligence score (Fig. 2). Pair-wise comparison at each gestational age showed that the difference in cognitive scores increased as the time interval between gestational weeks got longer (Additional file 1: Fig. S2). Compared to adolescents born at ≥ 40 weeks, adolescents born at less than 33 weeks, 34–35 weeks, and 36 weeks showed significantly lower total intelligence cognitive scores (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected) (presented as effect size using Cohen’s d with 95% CI) (d =  − 0.44, − 0.56 to − 0.31; d =  − 0.16, − 0.25 to − 0.08; d =  − 0.14, − 0.22 to − 0.06) and crystallized intelligence score (d =  − 0.44, − 0.56 to − 0.31; d =  − 0.20, − 0.29 to − 0.12; d =  − 0.11, − 0.19 to − 0.02). For the fluid intelligence score, only adolescents of less than 33 weeks (d =  − 0.29, − 0.41 to − 0.16) and 36 weeks (d =  − 0.12, − 0.2 to − 0.04) showed significantly lower cognitive performance. Additionally, compared to adolescents born at ≥ 40 weeks, adolescents born at ≤ 28 weeks and 29 to 33 weeks also showed a lower total intelligence score (d =  − 0.39, − 0.65 to − 0.14; d =  − 0.30, − 0.4 to − 0.2), crystallized intelligence score (d =  − 0.42, − 0.67 to − 0.16; d =  − 0.31, − 0.41 to − 0.21) and fluid intelligence score (d =  − 0.39, − 0.65 to − 0.14; d =  − 0.19, − 0.36 to − 0.16). There were no significant differences in cognitive performance between adolescents born at ≤ 28 weeks and those born at 29 to 33 weeks (total intelligence score: d =  − 0.15, − 0.43 to 0.12; crystallized intelligence score: d =  − 0.06, − 0.34 to 0.21; fluid intelligence score: d =  − 0.14, − 0.41 to 0.13). Table S2 presents the statistical results for the subscales. Intriguingly, cognitive differences between gestational groups did not differ by sex (interaction P > 0.3). Further, lower cognitive performances on other measuring tools, including the Little Man Task, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, and the Matrix Reasoning Task were associated with in a graded way with lower gestational age toward extremely preterm (as shown in Additional file 1: Table S3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Significant group difference of gestational age on cognitive performance in 11,847 adolescents at baseline. All displayed cognitive measurements are corrected by Bonferroni multiple comparisons at 0.05 level in the linear mixed model analysis. The Y-axis is the cognitive score (from the abcd_tbss), and the error bar is the standard deviation. Asterisks indicate that the corresponding cognitive scores of each gestational age group differ from that in the 40-week gestational group at P < 0.001(***), P < 0.01(**), or P < 0.05(*) (all Bonferroni corrected). Age, sex, body mass index, family income, parental education year, race, puberty score, maternal age at delivery, and prenatal exposure to any kind of substance before or after maternal knowledge of pregnancy were regressed out in all analyses. Note: nihtbx_totalcomp_fc: Cognition Total Composite Score Fully-Corrected T-score; nihtbx_cryst_fc: Crystallized Composite Fully-Corrected T-score; nihtbx_fluidcomp_fc: Cognition Fluid Composite Fully-Corrected T-score; nihtbx_reading_fc: NIH Toolbox Oral Reading Recognition Test Age 3 + v2.0 Fully-Corrected T-score; nihtbx_picvocab_fc: NIH Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Test Age 3 + v2.0 Fully-Corrected T-score; nihtbx_list_fc: NIH Toolbox List Sorting Working Memory Test Age 7 + v2.0 Fully-Corrected T-score; nihtbx_picture_fc: NIH Toolbox Picture Sequence Memory Test Age 8 + Form A v2.0 Fully-Corrected T-score. The Fully-Corrected T-score in the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery was a type of standardized score based on a score distribution that has a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10

Similar results were observed at the age of 11–12, namely that differences of cognitive performance were associated with gestational age (GA) (P < 0.05 to P < 0.01, Bonferroni corrected; Table S4). A positive association was found between GA and the crystallized intelligence score (β = 0.37/week; 95% CI, 0.25–0.48/week). For every 1 week longer of gestational duration, the crystallized intelligence score was higher by 0.74%. Compared to adolescents born at 40 weeks, adolescents born at less than 33 weeks, 34–35 weeks, and 36 weeks showed significantly lower crystallized intelligence scores (d =  − 0.31, − 0.44 to − 0.18; d =  − 0.19, − 0.30 to − 0.08; d =  − 0.13, − 0.24 to − 0.02). For total and fluid intelligence scores, there were no results because of missing data. The statistical results on the sub-domains of cognitive abilities were presented in Additional file 1: Table S4.

Younger gestational age is associated with lower volumes of brain regions at 9–10 years

Lower cortical and subcortical volumes found within the five gestational groups were primarily located in fronto-parieto-temporal areas, the fusiform gyrus, cingulate cortex, insula, postcentral gyrus and the right hippocampus, thalamus, and pallidum (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected; Fig. 3A). Lower volumes of some brain regions were associated in a graded way with lower gestational age toward extremely preterm (Additional file 1: Fig. S3). Indeed, total brain volume at adolescence was positively correlated with gestational age (d = 0.12, 95% CI: 0.98 to 1.03; Fig. S4). Specifically, compared to 40-week term peers, adolescents born at 36 weeks and 37–39 weeks showed the fewest brain regions with lower volume, and these brain regions involved the inferior frontal gyrus pars triangularis (BA45) and the postcentral gyrus (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected; Fig. 3B). The adolescents born at 34–35 weeks had more brain regions with lower volumes, and these were located mainly in parieto-temporal areas (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected; Fig. 3B). For adolescents born at ≤ 33 weeks, more brain regions had lower volumes, including also other parieto-temporal regions, with, in addition, frontal regions, the fusiform gyrus, cingulate cortex, insula, postcentral gyrus and the right hippocampus, thalamus, and pallidum (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected; Fig. 3B). The details of these paired comparisons are provided in Table S5. Further, the group differences related to gestational age remained similar when adolescents born with extreme birth weights (that is, large or small for gestational age) were excluded (Table S8) or when adolescents with a cesarean birth were excluded (Table S9). In addition, the group differences were not related to family income for any brain regions (interaction P > 0.05, Bonferroni corrected; Table S10).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Significant group differences of brain volume for different gestational ages in 11,203 adolescents at baseline. Brain map parcellation is according to the Desikan-Killiany atlas and regions are outlined in black. A Significant group differences of brain volume for different gestational age measured by a one-way ANOVA following the linear mixed model. The color represents the F value (Bonferroni corrected, P < 0.05). B Pairwise comparisons of the volumes of brain regions for adolescents in each group of gestational age and those at 40 weeks with a statistical threshold. The color represents the Cohen’s d value (Bonferroni correction, P < 0.05). L: left, R: right; Bon. cor.: Bonferroni correction. The surface visualization was generated using the R package ggseg (https://github.com/ggseg/ggseg)

Brain volume mediates the association between gestational age and cognitive function

Mediation analysis showed that the indirect effect of the gestational age on the cognition total intelligence score was significantly mediated by the mean cortical volume of the significant brain regions shown in Fig. 3A (Path AB, 13.8% of the total effect size measured by the variance explained (VE), β = 0.008, 95% CI: 0.005 to 0.01, P = 1.5 × 10−4, Fig. 4A). Similar mediation effects were also found for the crystallized and fluid intelligence scores (Path AB, VE = 20%, β = 0.01, 95% CI: 0.007 to 0.012, P = 1.5 × 10−4, Fig. 4B; Path AB, VE = 6.67%, β = 0.003, 95% CI: 0.002 to 0.005, P = 1.4 × 10−4, Fig. 4C). The mediation results for the volume of specific brain region are shown in Table S6.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Cortical volume mediates the association between gestational age and cognitive assessments in 11,203 adolescents at baseline. The standardized mediations by cortical volume were significant from the gestational age on total intelligence (A β = 0.01, P = 1.51 × 10−4), crystallized intelligence (B β = 0.01, P = 1.50 × 10−4), and fluid intelligence (C β = 0.003, P = 1.37 × 10−4). Path A: the effect of the independent variable (gestational age) on the mediator (cortical volume); Path B: the effect of the mediator (cortical volume) on the dependent variable (cognitive intelligence); Path C: the regression coefficient (β value) representing the total effect of the independent variable (the gestational age) on the dependent variable (cognitive intelligence) when the mediator (cortical volume) was not taken into account; Path C’: the direct effect of the gestational age on the cognitive intelligence when controlling for the cortical volume. The regression coefficient in Path C’ shows some reduction in contrast to that in Path C. Path AB: the indirect effect of gestational age on cognitive intelligence through cortical volume can then be quantified as the product of Path A multiply by Path B. CI: confidence interval. The cortical volume used as mediator was the averaged value across regions showing significant group difference of gestational age at baseline

Longitudinal changes of brain volume from 9–10 to 11–12 years

For the brain regions showing significant between-group volume differences at baseline (as shown in Fig. 3A), the main effects remained at the follow-up, with most of those with lower gestational age still showing lower brain volumes at the follow-up, despite an overall trend to lower brain volume from baseline to follow-up (Fig. 5A and Fig. 6). Further, no significant group-by-time interactions were found for the volume of each brain region or the total brain volume (Fig. 5, Fig. 6 and Table S7), and the same findings for subcortical/non-neocortical brain regions (Fig. S5 and Table S7).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Cortical volume changes from 9–10 years to 11–12 years in 5296 adolescents. A Averaged brain volume in regions shown in Fig. 3A presented no significant group-by-time interaction. B No significant interactions of time on group differences for each region shown in Fig. 3A. The color represents the F value

Fig. 6
figure 6

Cortical volume for each brain region changes from 9–10 years to 11–12 years in 5296 adolescents. Dot plot for regions showing no significant group by time interaction within the two-year follow-up period. banksstsrh, banks of superior temporal sulcus; L, Left; R, Right

External validation using the Danish nationwide cohort study

Analysis of data from the Danish nationwide cohort [26] showed that adolescents’ lower gestational age was associated with a lower proportion of adolescents taking the final examination (Additional file 1: Fig. S6) and lower scores for all educational domains, including problem-solving abilities and skills in Mathematics, and oral skill, reading comprehension, and spelling in Danish (Table 2, N = 618,070). For every week of a longer gestational duration, the problem-solving abilities, mathematical skills, oral, reading comprehension, and spelling scores go up by 0.5%, 0.6%, 0.2%, 0.5%, and 0.3%, respectively. For example, in Mathematics, we observed a positive association between GA and standardized problem-solving abilities (β = 0.009; 95% CI, 0.007 to 0.011). Compared to children born after 40 weeks, significantly lower scores for problem-solving abilities were observed for adolescents born less than 33 weeks (β =  − 0.141; 95% CI, − 0.166 to − 0.117), 34–35 weeks (β =  − 0.039; 95 CI%, − 0.060 to − 0.019), 36 weeks (β =  − 0.025; 95% CI, − 0.044 to − 0.007), and 37–39 weeks (β = 0.012; 95% CI, − 0.017 to − 0.006), respectively. Similar patterns were also observed in other academic domains, including mathematical skills, oral, reading comprehension, and spelling scores (as shown in Table 2 and Additional file 1: Results).

Table 2 Difference in standardized mean examination scores according to gestational weeks from the Danish cohort study

Discussion

This large population-based cohort study describes the relationships between gestational age and in adolescence the volume of different brain regions, cognition, and educational performance. It was found that shorter gestational age is associated in early adolescence with lower cortical volume primarily in fronto-parieto-temporal areas and lower multidimensional cognitive functions (Age, sex, body mass index, family income, parental education year, race, puberty score, maternal age at delivery, and prenatal exposure to any kind of substance before or after maternal knowledge of pregnancy were regressed out in all analyses.) Moreover, it was shown that the association between younger gestational age and lower cognitive function in early adolescence was partly mediated by the lower cortical volumes that were found. The longitudinal analysis showed that there was a decrease in the volumes of brain regions from baseline until the 2-year follow-up, but the main effects remained, with those of lower gestational age still showing lower brain volumes at the 2-year follow-up at 11–12 years. The differences of cognition for different gestational ages were in the order of 8% (Fig. 2), and the differences of brain volume in the order of 2% (Fig. 5). Analysis of data from the Danish nationwide cohort [26] provided validation of the results in that lower gestational age was associated in middle adolescents with lower scores for all educational domains (n = 618,070, all P < 0.003).

The findings extend previous studies of lower cognitive function (most of them are IQs) or poorer educational performance in very preterm or moderate preterm children [52,53,54,55,56], by providing evidence for more refined categories of gestational age using a large sample size (n = 11,878 from the ABCD cohort) together with cross-validation (n = 618,070 Denmark cohort), and also multi-dimensional measures of cognition, and school achievements. There were significantly lower school achievements even in the 37–39 week group compared to the full term of 40-week group that lasted until middle adolescence. This suggests that shorter gestational duration is associated with lower educational performance that is still present until at least middle adolescence.

The neuroimaging findings extend previous studies by showing that each preterm gestational group has a lower brain volume at adolescence and that lower volume is closely related to lower gestational age (Fig. 3) [57]. The graded smaller brain volumes described here for earlier gestational ages (Fig. 3B) are consistent with a recent study that treated gestational age as a continuous variable and demonstrated a positive correlation with total brain volume at 10 years [22]. Indeed, we found that total brain volume at adolescence is linearly positively correlated with gestational age (d = 0.12, 95% CI: 0.98 to1.03, P = 5.6 × 10−11). Here, we extend this correlation and relate lower brain volume to specific gestational age groups. We found that adolescents born at less than 36 weeks of gestation showed a wide range of lower brain volumes and that this range decreased sharply after 36 weeks, suggesting that 36 weeks may be a key point. This graded pattern was confirmed in our cognitive findings. Consistent with previous studies that frequently reported alterations in cortical gyrification, cortical thickness, surface area, and structural covariance networks in preterm youths or adults [17, 18, 21], we also found that adolescents born at less than 36 weeks presented with lower volume in temporal lobe regions. These findings suggest that children born during the third trimester may have structural differences in the temporal lobe region, one of the latest brain regions to mature [58, 59], where synaptogenesis and gyrification begin and progress rapidly throughout the third trimester [60,61,62]. In addition, the lower volumes in the prefrontal and parietal areas were predominantly present in adolescents born before 33 weeks’ gestation and were no longer significant in adolescents born after 33 weeks’ gestation. Our findings in this large cohort thus suggest that different gestational ages may be associated with differences in different brain regions. A previous study also found that the volume in the prefrontal region was lower in a cohort of 29 preterm (< 30 weeks) children at 12 years [63]. Further, lower brain volumes in preterm adolescents may reflect less brain development in utero and/or underdevelopment during postnatal maturation [58].

The brain regions with lower volume associated with lower gestational age are illustrated in Figs. 3 and 5 and listed in Table S5. The lateral temporal lobe regions include cortex in the banks of the superior temporal sulcus that is involved in representing socially relevant visual stimuli such as face expression and movements [64,65,66,67] and in the semantic representations involved in language [68]. The inferior prefrontal regions including Broca’s area and the temporal-parietal regions are also implicated in language [68]. The postcentral gyrus and connected insula and inferior and medial parietal cortex regions are involved in somatosensory processing and representing actions in space [69,70,71]. The hippocampal regions are involved in memory [72,73,74]. The orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex are involved in emotion [75, 76].

A previously unanswered critical question is the underlying mechanism in the perinatal period that affects the neurodevelopmental risks. The present study advances previous research by identifying brain regions the volumes of which significantly mediate the association between GA and cognitive function. In addition, longitudinal analysis revealed that the lower cortical volumes found in preterm groups are consistent with typical maturational processes around adolescence [77,78,79]. For instance, one study showed a decline in brain volumes from 8 to12 years [8] and another study using a relatively larger sample size also found volumetric decrease from 8 to 23 years [3]. Various biological mechanisms are proposed to underlie lower cortical volumes, including slowed cell growth, decreased dendritic arbor size, and elimination of synapses [61]. Another key finding here is that even by age 11–12, the lower GA groups still had lower cortical volumes. The trajectories described here are consistent with those found in recent studies on cortical volume in very preterm adolescents (< 30 weeks’ gestation) or of very low birthweight (< 1250 g) children [19]. A similar developmental pattern was also found for cortical thickness and surface area in preterm children and adolescents (< 37 gestational weeks) with very low birth weight [20]. All these evidences may suggest a “lack of catch-up” of brain structural growth in the preterm groups, which potentially reflects an altered brain developmental route following prenatal maturational differences, and supports the view that brain development in the postnatal years after premature birth may not compensate [80].

Strengths and limitations

Several strengths of the research are described here. First, this is a large-scale (n = 11,878), retrospective cohort study characterized by refined categories of gestational weeks and comprehensive covariate data integrating with cognitive assessments. The longitudinal design for the study enabled us to track the growth trajectories two years later in brain regions with their volumes significantly associated with gestational age and cognition. Second, in addition to regressing out a variety of prenatal and postnatal covariates related to family variables, pregnancy, and adolescence (see the “ Methods” section), we also considered variables closely related to gestational age (i.e., birth weight and cesarean delivery), providing a reasonable and rigorous model for exploring the association between gestational age and neural and cognitive development until adolescence. Third, the nationwide setting of this database and further validation with a much larger sample size from the Danish cohort (n = 618,070) enabled the association between gestational age and adolescent development to be extended from research tests to educational performance.

Several limitations need to be considered. First, causal relationships cannot be inferred from our retrospective cohort design, which is primarily an association study. The exact biological mechanism underlying the association between gestational age and postnatal maturation needs to be further explored. Second, recall bias should be considered because detailed gestational age information during pregnancy was based on questionnaires in this database. For example, the current ABCD database defined term birth as having 40 weeks of gestation which may include a small portion of participants born at greater than 40 weeks. Further exploration is needed to examine a wider range of gestational age from preterm to post-term gestation (i.e., > 40 weeks). Third, the current categories of gestational age are not based on each gestational week. Certain gestational weeks were grouped together due to smaller number of participants born for some gestational ages. Therefore, it limited our understanding of how the brain develops in early adolescence with week-to-week gestations. Fourth, complete data for 2-year follow-up cognitive abilities are not available, which makes exploration of the longitudinal development of cognitive performance not possible. But at least, for the only available data of crystallized intelligence scores, we found lower cognitive performance still present in preterm groups at 11–12 years, suggesting that cognitive differences may also be long-lasting. Fifth, although we tried to control for as many relevant variables as possible, we could not control for every possible variable. In future studies, if available, more variables related to prematurity and adolescents’ cognitive functioning could also be considered, such as pre- and postnatal nutrition and access to health care. Sixth, the effect size for lower brain volume was smaller in the near-term preterm group than in the earlier preterm groups, so for preterm adolescents born close to the full-term week, the clinical significance may be lower. However, brain volume in all preterm groups was stably lower than that in the term group at both 9–10 years and at the 2-year later follow-up. Lastly, the participants of this retrospective cohort study were extracted from a database from the United States, and therefore, the generalization to other countries and areas requires further validation.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this study provides new large-scale evidence about how lower gestational ages are associated at adolescence with lower volumes of some brain regions and with lower cognition. This relation between gestational age and subsequent school achievements was further demonstrated in a very large Danish cohort (n = 618,070). The study indicates the importance of early screening and close follow-up for neurocognitive and behavioral development for children and adolescents born with gestational ages that are even a little lower than full term.

Availability of data and materials

Neuroimaging and cognitive data from the ABCD dataset are available from.

https://nda.nih.gov/abcd with the approval of the ABCD consortium. The datasets for Danish cohort are not open access.

Abbreviations

ABCD:

Adolescent brain cognitive development

ASEG:

Automatic subcortical segmentation

GA:

Gestational age

LGA:

Large for gestational age

LMM:

Linear mixed model

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

SGA:

Small for gestational age

References

  1. Raznahan A, Greenstein D, Lee NR, Clasen LS, Giedd JN. Prenatal growth in humans and postnatal brain maturation into late adolescence. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109(28):11366–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Bethlehem RAI, Seidlitz J, White SR, Vogel JW, Anderson KM, Adamson C, et al. Brain charts for the human lifespan. Nature. 2022;604:525–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Gennatas ED, Avants BB, Wolf DH, Satterthwaite TD, Ruparel K, Ciric R, Hakonarson H, Gur RE, Gur RC. Age-related effects and sex differences in gray matter density, volume, mass, and cortical thickness from childhood to young adulthood. J Neurosci. 2017;37(20):5065–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Clouchoux C, Guizard N, Evans AC, du Plessis AJ, Limperopoulos C. Normative fetal brain growth by quantitative in vivo magnetic resonance imaging. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2012;206(2):173 (e171-173. e178).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Kostovic I, Vasung L. Insights from in vitro fetal magnetic resonance imaging of cerebral development. In: 2009: Semin Perinatol. 2009. p. 220–33.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Silva CCV, El Marroun H, Sammallahti S, Vernooij MW, Muetzel RL, Santos S, Jaddoe VWV. Patterns of fetal and infant growth and brain morphology at age 10 years. JAMA Netw Open. 2021;4(12):e2138214.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Peterson BS. Brain imaging studies of the anatomical and functional consequences of preterm birth for human brain development. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1008(1):219–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ment LR, Kesler S, Vohr B, Katz KH, Baumgartner H, Schneider KC, Delancy S, Silbereis J, Duncan CC, Constable RT, et al. Longitudinal brain volume changes in preterm and term control subjects during late childhood and adolescence. Pediatrics. 2009;123(2):503–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Crump C, Sundquist K, Sundquist J, Winkleby MA. Gestational age at birth and mortality in young adulthood. JAMA. 2011;306(11):1233–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Broekman BF, Wang C, Li Y, Rifkin-Graboi A, Saw SM, Chong Y-S, Kwek K, Gluckman PD, Fortier MV, Meaney MJ. Gestational age and neonatal brain microstructure in term born infants: a birth cohort study. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(12):e115229.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. O’Donnell KJ, Meaney MJ. Fetal origins of mental health: the developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(4):319–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Santos IS, Barros FC, Munhoz T, Matijasevich A. Gestational age at birth and behavioral problems from four to 11 years of age: birth cohort study. BMC Pediatr. 2017;17(1):184.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Perumal N, Manji KP, Darling AM, Kisenge RR, Kvestad I, Hysing M, Belinger DC, Urassa W, Strand TA, Duggan CP, et al. Gestational age, birth weight, and neurocognitive development in adolescents in Tanzania. J Pediatr. 2021;236:194–203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Berry MJ, Foster T, Rowe K, Robertson O, Robson B, Pierse N. Gestational age, health, and educational outcomes in adolescents. Pediatrics. 2018;142(5):e20181016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Twilhaar ES, Wade RM, De Kieviet JF, Van Goudoever JB, Van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J. Cognitive outcomes of children born extremely or very preterm since the 1990s and associated risk factors: a meta-analysis and meta-regression. JAMA pediatr. 2018;172(4):361–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Lemola S, Oser N, Urfer-Maurer N, Brand S, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Bechtel N, Grob A, Weber P, Datta AN. Effects of gestational age on brain volume and cognitive functions in generally healthy very preterm born children during school-age: a voxel-based morphometry study. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(8):e0183519.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Nassar R, Kaczkurkin AN, Xia CH, Sotiras A, Pehlivanova M, Moore TM, Garcia de La Garza A, Roalf DR, Rosen AFG, Lorch SA, et al. Gestational age is dimensionally associated with structural brain network abnormalities across development. Cereb Cortex. 2019;29(5):2102–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Hedderich DM, Bäuml JG, Berndt MT, Menegaux A, Scheef L, Daamen M, Zimmer C, Bartmann P, Boecker H, Wolke D. Aberrant gyrification contributes to the link between gestational age and adult IQ after premature birth. Brain. 2019;142(5):1255–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Loh WY, Anderson PJ, Cheong JLY, Spittle AJ, Chen J, Lee KJ, Molesworth C, Inder TE, Connelly A, Doyle LW, et al. Longitudinal growth of the basal ganglia and thalamus in very preterm children. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;14(4):998–1011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sripada K, Bjuland KJ, Sølsnes AE, Håberg AK, Grunewaldt KH, Løhaugen GC, Rimol LM, Skranes J. Trajectories of brain development in school-age children born preterm with very low birth weight. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):15553.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Thompson DK, Matthews LG, Alexander B, Lee KJ, Kelly CE, Adamson CL, Hunt RW, Cheong JLY, Spencer-Smith M, Neil JJ, et al. Tracking regional brain growth up to age 13 in children born term and very preterm. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):696.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. El Marroun H, Zou R, Leeuwenburg MF, Steegers EA, Reiss IK, Muetzel RL, Kushner SA, Tiemeier H. Association of gestational age at birth with brain morphometry. JAMA pediatr. 2020;174(12):1149–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Nagy Z, Ashburner J, Andersson J, Jbabdi S, Draganski B, Skare S, Böhm B, Smedler AC, Forssberg H, Lagercrantz H. Structural correlates of preterm birth in the adolescent brain. Pediatrics. 2009;124(5):e964-972.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Rogers CE, Barch DM, Sylvester CM, Pagliaccio D, Harms MP, Botteron KN, Luby JL. Altered gray matter volume and school age anxiety in children born late preterm. J Pediatr. 2014;165(5):928–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews Editorial Staff. NIH's Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Alcohol Res. 2018;39(1):97. https://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pmc/articles/PMC6104964/.

  26. Bliddal M, Broe A, Pottegård A, Olsen J, Langhoff-Roos J. The Danish Medical Birth Register. Eur J Epidemiol. 2018;33(1):27–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Casey BJ, Cannonier T, Conley MI, Cohen AO, Barch DM, Heitzeg MM, Soules ME, Teslovich T, Dellarco DV, Garavan H, et al. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:43–54. ABCD https://abcdstudy.org/scientists/data-sharing/.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Auchter AM, Hernandez Mejia M, Heyser CJ, Shilling PD, Jernigan TL, Brown SA, Tapert SF, Dowling GJ. A description of the ABCD organizational structure and communication framework. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:8–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Garavan H, Bartsch H, Conway K, Decastro A, Goldstein RZ, Heeringa S, Jernigan T, Potter A, Thompson W, Zahs D. Recruiting the ABCD sample: design considerations and procedures. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:16–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Organization WH. Born too soon: the global action report on preterm birth. 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Ask H, Gustavson K, Ystrom E, Havdahl KA, Tesli M, Askeland RB, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. Association of gestational age at birth with symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172(8):749–56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Mungas D, Widaman K, Zelazo PD, Tulsky D, Heaton RK, Slotkin J, Blitz DL, Gershon RC. VII. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): factor structure for 3 to 15 year olds. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2013;78(4):103–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Hagler DJ Jr, Hatton S, Cornejo MD, Makowski C, Fair DA, Dick AS, Sutherland MT, Casey BJ, Barch DM, Harms MP, et al. Image processing and analysis methods for the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. Neuroimage. 2019;202:116091.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):968–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33(3):341–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Wang H, Rolls ET, Du X, Du J, Yang D, Li J, Li F, Cheng W, Feng J. Severe nausea and vomiting in pregnancy: psychiatric and cognitive problems and brain structure in children. BMC Med. 2020;18(1):228.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Cheng W, Rolls E, Gong W, Du J, Zhang J, Zhang X-Y, Li F, Feng J. Sleep duration, brain structure, and psychiatric and cognitive problems in children. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;26(8):3992–4003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Lim SL, Padmala S, Pessoa L. Segregating the significant from the mundane on a moment-to-moment basis via direct and indirect amygdala contributions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(39):16841–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Alfons A, Ateş NY, Groenen PJ. A robust bootstrap test for mediation analysis. Organ Res Methods. 2018;25(3):591–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Kramer MS, Platt RW, Wen SW, Joseph K, Allen A, Abrahamowicz M, Blondel B, Bréart G, Fetal/Infant Health Study Group of the Canadian Perinatal Surveillance System. A new and improved population-based Canadian reference for birth weight for gestational age. Pediatrics. 2001;108(2):e35–e35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Zhang G, Feenstra B, Bacelis J, Liu X, Muglia LM, Juodakis J, Miller DE, Litterman N, Jiang P-P, Russell L. Genetic associations with gestational duration and spontaneous preterm birth. N Engl J Med. 2017;377(12):1156–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Judd N, Sauce B, Wiedenhoeft J, Tromp J, Chaarani B, Schliep A, van Noort B, Penttila J, Grimmer Y, Insensee C, et al. Cognitive and brain development is independently influenced by socioeconomic status and polygenic scores for educational attainment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117(22):12411–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Machlin L, McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA. Brain structure mediates the association between socioeconomic status and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev Sci. 2020;23(1):e12844.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Asano K, Asano M, Sassa Y, Yokota S, Kotozaki Y, Nouchi R, Kawashima R. Childhood socioeconomic status is associated with psychometric intelligence and microstructural brain development. Commun Biol. 2021;4(1):470.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Tikhomirova T, Malykh A, Malykh S. Predicting academic achievement with cognitive abilities: cross-sectional study across school education. Behav Sci (Basel). 2020;10(10):158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Schmidt M, Schmidt SA, Sandegaard JL, Ehrenstein V, Pedersen L, Sørensen HT. The Danish National Patient Registry: a review of content, data quality, and research potential. Clin Epidemiol. 2015;7:449–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Jensen VM, Rasmussen AW. Danish Education Registers. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):91–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Lynge E, Sandegaard JL, Rebolj M. The Danish National Patient Register. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):30–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Mors O, Perto GP, Mortensen PB. The Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):54–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Xia Y, Xiao J, Yu Y, Tseng W-L, Lebowitz E, DeWan AT, Pedersen LH, Olsen J, Li J, Liew Z. Rates of neuropsychiatric disorders and gestational age at birth in a Danish population. JAMA Netw Open. 2021;4(6):e2114913–e2114913.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Tran DT, Preen DB, Einarsdottir K, Kemp-Casey A, Randall D, Jorm LR, Choi SK, Havard A. Use of smoking cessation pharmacotherapies during pregnancy is not associated with increased risk of adverse pregnancy outcomes: a population-based cohort study. BMC Med. 2020;18(1):1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Lee ES, Yeatman JD, Luna B, Feldman HM. Specific language and reading skills in school-aged children and adolescents are associated with prematurity after controlling for IQ. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(5):906–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Loe IM, Lee ES, Luna B, Feldman HM. Behavior problems of 9–16 year old preterm children: biological, sociodemographic, and intellectual contributions. Early Hum Dev. 2011;87(4):247–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Jaekel J, Baumann N, Wolke D. Effects of gestational age at birth on cognitive performance: a function of cognitive workload demands. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(5):e65219.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. De Jong M, Verhoeven M, van Baar AL. School outcome, cognitive functioning, and behaviour problems in moderate and late preterm children and adults: a review. In: 2012: Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2012. p. 163–9.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Heuvelman H, Abel K, Wicks S, Gardner R, Johnstone E, Lee B, Magnusson C, Dalman C, Rai D. Gestational age at birth and risk of intellectual disability without a common genetic cause. Eur J Epidemiol. 2018;33(7):667–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(10):861–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Hill J, Inder T, Neil J, Dierker D, Harwell J, Van Essen D. Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(29):13135–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Prayer D, Kasprian G, Krampl E, Ulm B, Witzani L, Prayer L, Brugger PC. MRI of normal fetal brain development. Eur J Radiol. 2006;57(2):199–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. van der Knaap MS, van Wezel-Meijler G, Barth PG, Barkhof F, Adèr HJ, Valk J. Normal gyration and sulcation in preterm and term neonates: appearance on MR images. Radiology. 1996;200(2):389–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1997;387(2):167–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Davis EP, Buss C, Muftuler LT, Head K, Hasso A, Wing DA, Hobel C, Sandman CA. Children’s brain development benefits from longer gestation. Front Psychol. 2011;2:1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Kesler SR, Reiss AL, Vohr B, Watson C, Schneider KC, Katz KH, Maller-Kesselman J, Silbereis J, Constable RT, Makuch RW, et al. Brain volume reductions within multiple cognitive systems in male preterm children at age twelve. J Pediatr. 2008;152(4):513–20 (520.e511).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Rolls ET, Deco G, Huang C-C, Feng J. Multiple cortical visual streams in humans. Cereb Cortex. 2022. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/cercor/bhac1276.

  65. Hasselmo ME, Rolls ET, Baylis GC, Nalwa V. Object-centred encoding by face-selective neurons in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey. Exp Brain Res. 1989;75:417–29.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Hasselmo ME, Rolls ET, Baylis GC. The role of expression and identity in the face-selective responses of neurons in the temporal visual cortex of the monkey. Behav Brain Res. 1989;32(3):203–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Pitcher D, Ungerleider LG. Evidence for a third visual pathway specialized for social perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2021;25(2):100–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Rolls ET, Deco G, Huang C-C, Feng J. The human language effective connectome. Neuroimage. 2022;258:119352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Rolls ET, Deco G, Huang C-C, Feng J. Prefrontal and somatosensory-motor cortex effective connectivity in humans. Cereb Cortex. 2022. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/cercor/bhac1391.

  70. Rolls ET, Deco G, Huang C-C, Feng J. The human posterior parietal cortex: effective connectome, and its relation to function. Cereb Cortex. 2022. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/cercor/bhac1266.

  71. Rolls ET, Wirth S, Deco G, Huang C-C, Feng J. The human posterior cingulate, retrosplenial and medial parietal cortex effective connectome, and implications for memory and navigation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2022: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/HBM.26089.

  72. Rolls ET, Deco G, Huang CC, Feng J. The effective connectivity of the human hippocampal memory system. Cereb Cortex. 2022;32:3706–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Rolls ET. Hippocampal spatial view cells for memory and navigation, and their underlying connectivity in humans. Hippocampus. 2022. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/HIPO.23467.

  74. Rolls ET. The hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and episodic and semantic memory. Prog Neurobiol. 2022;217:102334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Rolls ET, Deco G, Huang CC, Feng J. The human orbitofrontal cortex, vmPFC, and anterior cingulate cortex effective connectome: emotion, memory, and action. Cereb Cortex. 2022. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/cercor/bhac1070.

  76. Rolls ET, Cheng W, Feng J. The orbitofrontal cortex: reward, emotion, and depression. Brain Commun. 2020;2:fcaa196.

  77. Wilke M, Krägeloh-Mann I, Holland SK. Global and local development of gray and white matter volume in normal children and adolescents. Exp Brain Res. 2007;178(3):296–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Marsh R, Gerber AJ, Peterson BS. Neuroimaging studies of normal brain development and their relevance for understanding childhood neuropsychiatric disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(11):1233–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Swagerman S, Brouwer R, de Geus E, Hulshoff Pol H, Boomsma D. Development and heritability of subcortical brain volumes at ages 9 and 12. Genes Brain Behav. 2014;13(8):733–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Gale CR, O’Callaghan FJ, Bredow M, Martyn CN. The influence of head growth in fetal life, infancy, and childhood on intelligence at the ages of 4 and 8 years. Pediatrics. 2006;118(4):1486–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/.

ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented that study and/or provided data but did not participate in the analysis for or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators.

Funding

Qing Ma was supported by grants from National Postdoctoral Foundation of China (No. 2021M690700) and Shanghai Postdoctoral Excellence Program (No. 2020045). Wei Cheng was supported by grants from the National Natural Sciences Foundation of China (No. 82071997) and the Shanghai Rising Star Program (No. 21QA1408700). Fei Li was supported by grants from National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 82125032, No. 81930095, and No. 81761128035), the Science and Technology Commission of Shanghai Municipality (No. 19410713500 and No. 2018SHZDZX01), the Shanghai Municipal Commission of Health and Family Planning (No. GWV-10.1-XK07, No. 2020CXJQ01, No. 2018YJRC03), Innovative research team of high-level local universities in Shanghai (SHSMU-ZDCX20211100), and the Guangdong Key Project (No. 2018B030335001). Hui Wang was supported by grants from National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 82273646) and China Medical Broad Open Competition Program (No. 21-418).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Q.M., H.W., and J.L. analyzed data from the ABCD cohort or data from the Danish cohort and drafted the corresponding content. E.T.R. advised extensively on the interpretation of the findings and rewrote the paper. S.T.X., Y.Z.L., and Q.J.Z. helped check the data. W.C. guided and helped data analysis and paper writing. F.L. proposed the study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Wei Cheng or Fei Li.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

In the ABCD study, all procedures were approved by a central Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of California, San Diego, and in some cases by individual site IRBs (e.g., Washington University in St. Louis) (https://0-www-sciencedirect-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/science/article/pii/S1878929317300622). Parents or guardians provided written informed consent after the procedures had been fully explained and children assented before participation in the study. In the Danish national study, the study was approved by the Data Protection Agency (record number 2013–41-2569). By Danish law, no informed consent is required for a register-based study of anonymized data.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Demographic characteristics of adolescents atninth-grade born with different gestational weeks from the Danish cohort study.Table S2. Group difference for gestational age and cognitive measures at 9-10 years. Table S3. Validation of group difference for gestational age andcognitive measures at 9-10 years. Table S4. Group difference for gestational age and cognitive measures at 11-12years. Table S5. Groupdifference for the association between gestational age and brain volume in adolescents at 9-10 years. Table S6.Mediation result for mediation by each volumetric region of the association between gestational age and cognitive measures in adolescents at 9-10 years. Table S7. Longitudinal group-by-time interaction for brain volume in adolescents from 9-10 years to 11-12 years. Table S8. Group difference for theassociation between gestational age and brain volume when excluding adolescentswith extreme birth weight at 9-10 years. Table S9. Group difference for the association between gestationalage and brain volume at 9-10 years when excluding adolescents with Caesareian birth. Table S10. Group-by-income interaction for brain volumes in adolescents at 9-10 years. Fig. S1. The distribution of gestational weeks at both baseline and 2-years follow-up neuroimaging analyses.Fig. S2. Differences of cognitive performance between any two gestational weeks. Fig. S3. Brain regions with lower volumes for different gestational age. Fig. S4. Positive correlation between gestational age and whole brain volumes at baseline. Fig. S5.Subcortical/non-neocortical volume change from 9-10 years to 11-12 years. Fig. S6. Proportion of adolescents taking the final examination according to gestational weeks from the Danish cohort study.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ma, Q., Wang, H., Rolls, E.T. et al. Lower gestational age is associated with lower cortical volume and cognitive and educational performance in adolescence. BMC Med 20, 424 (2022). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12916-022-02627-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12916-022-02627-3

Keywords