Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Short stature and language development in the United Kingdom: a longitudinal analysis of children from the Millennium Cohort Study

Abstract

Background

In low- and middle-income countries, poverty and impaired growth prevent children from meeting their cognitive developmental potential. There are few studies investigating these relationships in high-income settings.

Methods

Participants were 12,536 children born between 2000 and 2002 in the UK and participating in the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS). Short stature was defined as having a height-for-age 2 or more standard deviations below the median (≤ − 2 SDS) at age 3 years. Standardized British Abilities Scales II (BAS II) language measures, used to assess language development at ages 3, 5, 7 and 11 years, were the main outcome assessed.

Results

Children with short stature at age 3 years (4.1%) had language development scores that were consistently lower from ages 3 to 11 years (− 0.26 standard deviations (SD) (95% CI − 0.37, − 0.15)). This effect was attenuated but remained significant after adjustment for covariates. Trajectory analysis produced four distinct patterns of language development scores (low-declining, low-improving, average and high). Multinomial logistic regression models showed that children with short stature had a higher risk of being in the low-declining group, relative to the average group (relative risk ratio (RRR) = 2.11 (95% CI 1.51, 2.95)). They were also less likely to be in the high-scoring group (RRR = 0.65 (0.52, 0.82)). Children with short stature at age 3 years who had ‘caught up’ by age 5 years (height-for-age ≥ 2 SDS) did not have significantly different scores from children with persistent short stature, but had a higher probability of being in the high-performing group than children without catch-up growth (RRR = 1.84 (1.11, 3.07)).

Conclusions

Short stature at age 3 years was associated with lower language development scores at ages 3 to 11 years in UK children. These associations remained significant after adjustment for socioeconomic, child and parental factors.

Peer Review reports

Background

Globally, children living in poverty experience impaired linear growth. Impaired growth that results in a height-for-age more than 2 standard deviations below the World Health Organization Child Growth Standards median, termed short stature or stunting, is evident in almost a quarter of children globally [1]. Although stunting is most prevalent in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs), there is increasing recognition that linear growth failure is also a marker of socioeconomic disadvantage in high-income countries (HICs) such as the United Kingdom (UK) [2,3,4].

In LMICs, it has been estimated that poverty and stunting prevent over 200 million children from meeting their potential for cognitive, motor and social-emotional development [5]. Data from the Young Lives study in Ethiopia, India, Peru and Vietnam have shown that linear growth from the age of 6 months is positively associated with cognition from childhood to adolescence [6,7,8,9,10]. A recent meta-analysis of over 9000 participants from six prospective cohorts in Brazil, Guatemala, India, the Philippines and South Africa showed statistically significant associations between linear growth in the first 1000 days (from conception to a child’s second birthday) and both schooling attainment and intelligence quotient (IQ) in adulthood [11]. This meta-analysis reinforces similar findings over the last 15 years from several other LMICs [12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22].

Given the global public health focus on countries with the highest burden of stunting, little is known about the long-term impact of poor early-life linear growth in HICs such as the UK. Longitudinal positive associations of growth and cognition have been described in HICs, but most data come from historical British birth cohorts of children born in the 1940s, which limits the applicability of these studies to current policy and clinical practice [23, 24]. Here, we use longitudinal data from the UK Millennium Cohort Study (MCS), a cohort of children born in the UK in 2000–2002. We model verbal and language development from age 3 to age 11 to assess whether short stature is associated with impaired language development in a contemporary high-income setting.

Methods

Sample

The MCS is an ongoing cohort study of children born between 2000 and 2002 in the UK [25,26,27,28,29,30,31]. The study collected data on 18,294 singleton children at the first timepoint (2000–2002). A total of 15,406 children had height and development data at two or more timepoints and were therefore eligible for analysis. A sample flowchart is presented in Additional file 1: Fig. 1. Baseline data were collected at 9 months of age (or 3 years for children not present at timepoint 1) and children were followed longitudinally with repeat data collection at ages 3, 5, 7, 11, 14 and 17 years. A stratified sampling design was used to select areas of residence (electoral wards) from which all parents of children born within the date range were invited to participate. The sampling strategy oversampled children born in ethnically diverse and economically disadvantaged areas, to account for higher attrition in these families [32]. Each data collection timepoint included an interview with the main parent (who is self-identified and is usually the mother), the parent’s resident partner if applicable and the child (starting from age 7) [32]. Child height was measured at each visit to the nearest 0.1 cm using the Leicester Measure Stadiometer (Seca Ltd, Birmingham, UK). From age 3 years, trained fieldworkers assessed verbal, language and communication abilities; numeracy; problem solving; spatial awareness; and overall school readiness using a range of validated assessments at each timepoint. The current study used data from children at ages 3, 5, 7 and 11 years, with the timepoint at 3 years old as the baseline, as well as supplemental analyses of children aged 14 and 17 years.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Conceptual model of the relationship between short stature and language development

Data from the MCS are accessible on request from the UK Data Service. Ethical approval for the MCS was granted by the NHS Multi-Centre Research Ethics Committee (MREC). Integrated Research Application System (IRAS) approval for the current analysis was not required.

Measures

We used data from the British Abilities Scales II (BAS II), a battery of validated measures of verbal and language development at ages 3, 5, 7 and 11 years [33], which have been used in similar settings in the past [34]. The four timepoints available for these measures allowed us to model language development over time. These included (i) naming vocabulary (age 3 and 5 years), which assesses the child’s ability to correctly name pictures from a book; (ii) word reading (age 7 years), in which the child is asked to read aloud from a list of increasingly difficult words; and (iii) verbal similarities (age 11 years), which assesses the child’s ability to understand and describe similarities between concepts. BAS II scores are described within the MCS as age-standardised ability scores. To compare across timepoints, we further standardised these scores to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1. This approach has been used previously with these data [34].

The exposure variable was short stature at age 3 years, defined as a height-for-age more than 2 standard deviations (SDS) below the population median, using UK-WHO age- and sex-specific references [35]. A broad set of variables previously reported to be associated with growth and cognitive development were examined using a directed acyclic graph (DAG; Additional file 1: Fig. 2). A parsimonious model which included child and parent characteristics available in the dataset was selected from the full set of factors presented in the DAG (Fig. 1) [3, 36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Four trajectory groups of language development between 3 and 11 years of age (n = 12,536). Note: dots at each timepoint reflect the within-group mean. Lines denote the within-group trajectory. The best model fit was obtained using two linear polynomic functions (groups 1 and 4) and two cubic functions (groups 2 and 3)

Child characteristics

Sex was collected for all children at the first interview. Birth weight in kilogrammes was collected through birth registration records. Ethnicity was recorded during the first parental interview and was coded as White ethnicity or other ethnicities, due to small numbers in non-White ethnic groups. Age was accounted for in the standardised language development and height measures. It was also included as a time variable in each model.

Children with a number of growth-affecting conditions (see Additional file 1: Table 1) were excluded from primary analyses. Growth-affecting conditions were obtained from the child’s GP through data linkage, conducted in-house at MCS, and coded using the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) code.

Parental characteristics

Mid-parental height was included to account for the child’s genetic height potential. Mid-parental height is calculated as the mean of mother’s and father’s standardised heights using British 1990 references [54]. Where a child had missing values for maternal or paternal height, an adjustment formula was used to impute mid-parental height based on one parental height ((available parental height z score × 0.4) + 0.15). This formula was derived from MCS data using the relationship between both maternal and paternal height z score and child’s height z score at age 17 years. It accounts for both the regression to the mean expected in child height compared to parental heights and the increased variance when using one parental height z score instead of the mean of both. We included mother’s age at birth of the child, as well as second-hand smoke exposure (“Does smoke in the same room as [child] nowadays?”) and any breastfeeding (“Did [mother] ever try to breastfeed [child]?”). Whether the mother was ever diagnosed with depression or serious anxiety was included to account for maternal mental health.

Parental socioeconomic status was assessed with a range of variables. Maternal education was included as a binary variable, with mothers who had achieved level 3 education (A levels or vocational qualification) [55] or higher categorised as having a high level of education. A levels are completed in the UK at age 18, and 34% of UK students achieved at least one A level in 1995–1996 [56]. The Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) is derived annually by the Office for National Statistics as a measure of small-area relative deprivation for England, Wales, Scotland and Northern Ireland [57]. Census data on seven measures (including employment, access to healthcare and environment) are compiled to create the index. In the MCS, the child’s postcode was used to provide IMD deciles, with children in the first decile being the most deprived and those in the 10th decile being the least deprived. The IMD was treated as continuous in these analyses. Finally, we included an imputed measure of income quintile available in the MCS dataset. Missing income values were estimated using interval regression and socioeconomic predictor variables [58].

Analysis

Differences in sample characteristics by short stature were initially examined using chi-squared tests, t tests and ordinary least squares regression. We used two types of longitudinal growth models to assess the relationship between short stature at age 3 years and ongoing language development. Both methods used all available data, reducing the potential bias from attrition. For the current analyses, which modelled language development over time, we include participants with language development observations at two or more timepoints to adequately represent change over time.

We first assessed average language development scores over time by fitting mixed effects growth curve models using the mixed command in Stata 17 (StataCorp. 2021). Mixed effects models account for the highly correlated nature of repeated measures within individuals by fitting both random and fixed effects. Short stature, timepoint and covariates were included as fixed effects. Random intercept and slope effects were included by including individuals and time in the random part of the model. Time-squared and time-cubed effects were also tested as fixed effects to identify potential non-linearity in the data. Interaction terms between timepoint and short stature were included to assess whether the association with short stature changed over time. Model fit was assessed using likelihood ratio tests and assessing p values for individual effects. Short stature was then included in the best fitting model alone and adjusted for covariates.

We then performed group-based trajectory analysis using the traj user-written command in Stata [59]. Trajectory analysis uses maximum likelihood estimation to group individuals based on the similarity of their trajectories over time on the variable of interest. We used age in months as the time variable and first modelled the full sample with language development available at two or more timepoints. We used censored normal models to test the fit for 2, 3, 4 and 5 trajectory groups. We also tested the inclusion of linear, quadratic and cubic time effects. Model fit was evaluated using the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), as well as high posterior probabilities (above 0.7) and model interpretability. We then included short stature and covariates as risk in these models, as well as analysing the probability of being in each trajectory group through multinomial regression models with relative risk ratios (RRR).

Finally, we tested whether catch-up growth at age 5 (defined as having short stature at age 3 but not at age 5) was associated with improved verbal and language scores. We generated a categorical variable which included children who had never had short stature (height SDS >  − 2 at ages 3 and 5), those who had short stature at age 3 but had caught up by age 5 (height SDS ≤  − 2 at age 3, >  − 2 at age 5) and those who had persistent short stature (height SDS ≤  − 2 at ages 3 and 5). We included this variable in mixed effects models and trajectory models for language development from ages 5 to 11 years.

Missing data

We conducted complete case analyses; children missing height or covariate data at baseline or with less than two BAS II measurements were excluded. We checked for potential bias through an examination of the characteristics of included and excluded data. We conducted sensitivity analyses using multiple imputation (described in detail in Additional file 1: Text 1).

Sensitivity analyses

We tested a more stringent definition of short stature (≤ − 2.67 SDS), which is recommended in the UK to trigger referral for investigation into medical causes of short stature [60]. We also examined the association of height at age 3 with verbal and language development by fitting mixed effects models using a categorical variable to represent height SDS, grouping children with height-for-age of ≤  − 2 SDS, − 2 to − 1 SDS, − 1 to 0 SDS, 0 to 1 SDS, 1 to 2 SDS and > 2 SDS. A further sensitivity analysis used a later measure of language development at age 14 years in mixed effects models to assess whether the effects observed persisted into adolescence. The measure included at age 14 was the 1970 British Cohort Study Word Activity, which asks children to match a list of words to their correct synonyms. This measure was standardised in the same way as previous analyses. We did not use this measure in the main analyses as it is not part of the BAS II battery of tests. We further tested whether our results were sensitive to medical causes of short stature by excluding children who were identified as having any growth-affecting health condition (listed in Additional file 1: Table 1).

Supplemental analyses

We also conducted a supplemental analysis using a measure of mathematical ability at age 17 to assess whether associations could be observed into later adolescence. The measure included at age 17 was the number analogy activity which assesses basic arithmetic and reasoning with numbers and is scored out of ten. We used linear regression with standardised beta coefficients to model number analogy scores and their association with short stature at age 3 years. We again adjusted for all selected covariates.

Results

A total of 15,406 children (14,813 of 18,294 enrolled at timepoint 1 (81%), and 14,171 of 15,377 enrolled at timepoint 2 (92%)) had language development scores at two or more timepoints starting from the second wave of data collection at age 3 years; of these, 12,536 (81%) had baseline data for all covariates. Of the children included, 8799 had data for all four timepoints, with 3.9% of these children having short stature; 2474 children had data at three timepoints, with 4.9% of these children having short stature; and 1263 children had data at only two timepoints, with 6.6% of these children having short stature. A comparison of the included and excluded samples is presented in Additional file 1: Table 2, showing that there were some differences including lower BAS II scores and higher rates of short stature in the excluded sample.

A total of 514 (4.1%) children in the final sample had short stature (height SDS ≤  − 2). Sample characteristics by baseline short stature status are given in Table 1. Children with short stature had lower birth weight and younger mothers, were in a lower income quintile, lived in lower IMD areas, were less likely to have mothers with a high level of education and to have been breastfed and were more likely to be exposed to second-hand smoke.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics at age 3 years

Growth curve models

Growth curve models were fitted using the full sample (n = 15,406, Additional file 1: Table 3) and final sample (n = 12,536, Additional file 1: Table 4); these were largely consistent. The best fitting growth curve model included a linear fixed time effect (timepoint). Once included in the models, short stature was negatively associated with language development (Table 2). Children with short stature at age 3 years had language scores from ages 3 to 11 that were on average − 0.26 (95% CI − 0.37, − 0.15) standard deviations below those who did not have short stature. We also included an interaction term between short stature and timepoint, which showed no significant effect, suggesting that scores did not decline or improve over time for children with short stature compared to those without short stature.

Table 2 Verbal and language ability random slopes and random short stature mixed effects model including short stature and timepoint interaction effect (n = 12,536)

We fitted three adjusted models: the first adjusting only for sex, the second adjusting for sex and birth weight, and finally a fully adjusted model which included sex, birth weight, ethnicity, mid-parental height z score, mother’s age at birth of child, mother’s level of education, exposure to second-hand smoke, any breastfeeding, mother’s diagnosis of depression or anxiety, income quintile and IMD. These models showed that, whilst the effect of short stature was reduced when adjusting for covariates, short stature remained a significant predictor of poorer language development (Table 3).

Table 3 Verbal and language ability random slopes mixed effects models (n = 12,536)

Trajectory models

The same total sample with observations at 2 or more timepoints was used to fit an initial trajectory model (n = 15,406). A model with four trajectory classes and including linear and cubic coefficients had the best model fit. Model fit and characteristics are presented in Additional file 1: Fig. 3 and Additional file 1: Tables 5 and 6. We then fitted the same model using the final sample (n = 12,536). These models were very similar; therefore, we present the final sample model here.

The four trajectory classes identified are shown in Fig. 2. The first trajectory class had the smallest membership (4.8%) and identified children with low-declining scores. The second group also had a small membership (9.8%) and low but improving scores. The third group included children with high scores (30.0%). Finally, most children had scores which were consistently around the mean (average scores) (55.4%). Variability within each trajectory class is illustrated in random sampled spaghetti plots (Additional file 1: Fig. 4). Descriptive characteristics of the children in each of these groups are given in Table 4. There was a higher percentage of children with short stature in the low scores trajectories, and a lower percentage in the high-score trajectory.

Table 4 Sample characteristics at baseline by trajectory group membership (n = 12,536)

Covariates were added by adjusting for risk within the trajectory class model, as well as using multinomial regression models to model the probability of being in each trajectory class. We present multinomial regression models here and adjusted risk trajectory models in Additional file 1: Table 7. Both these approaches yielded largely consistent results. The first model, adjusted for sex, showed that short stature was associated with a higher risk of being in the low-performing classes and a lower risk of being in the high-performing class. These effects remained after adjustment, although the association between short stature and a higher probability of being in the low-improving class was weak and did not persist after full adjustment for covariates (Table 5).

Table 5 Multinomial logistic regression model of trajectory classes by short stature and covariates (n = 12,536)

Catch-up growth

Only 44.4% of children with height and full covariate data at ages 3 and 5 years who had short stature at age 3 had persistent short stature (1.8% of the whole cohort), whilst 55.6% had short stature at age 3 but not age 5 (2.2% of the whole cohort). A mixed effects model showed that catch-up growth at age 5 years was associated with some improvement in language development scores compared to no catch-up (Additional file 1: Table 8), although this effect did not reach significance (Table 6). Children with persistent short stature had lower verbal and language scores compared to children with no short stature and those who had caught up.

Table 6 Catch-up growth by age 5 years and language scores in children between 3 and 11 years of age (n = 11,860)

We also analysed catch-up growth using language development trajectory models. There were no significant differences in the probability of being in the low-score groups between children with and without catch-up growth. However, children with catch-up growth were significantly more likely than children without catch-up growth to be in the high-score group, although this association was attenuated after adjustment for all covariates (fully adjusted RRR = 1.65 [0.97, 2.80]) (Additional file 1: Table 9).

Sensitivity analyses

The results of sensitivity analyses were consistent with our main analyses. Analyses by height SDS cut-offs showed that the effect of short stature on verbal and language development was fully driven by children with height SDS ≤  − 2 (Additional file 1: Table 10). A visual inspection of height SDS and standardised language development showed that the relationship between both was largely driven by an increase in language scores with height up until around height SDS − 1.5, after which the association plateaued (Additional file 1: Fig. 5). There were 138 children with very short stature (height SDS ≤  − 2.67) in the sample (1.0%) and these had lower verbal and language development scores than taller children. These effects were larger than those for short stature (height SDS ≤  − 2) (a change of − 0.43 SD compared to − 0.26 SD) (Additional file 1: Table 11). The inclusion of a further timepoint at age 14 years produced results very consistent with the 3- to 11-year models (Additional file 1: Tables 12 and 13). Excluding the 1554 (12.4% of the final sample) children with growth-affecting health conditions did not change the inference of our findings (Additional file 1: Tables 14 and 15).

Multiple imputation models produced estimates which were consistent with our final results (Additional file 1: Tables 16, 17 and 18).

Supplemental analyses

We finally tested whether a measure of maths ability at age 17 years was associated with short stature at age 3 years (Additional file 1: Table 19). A total of 7686 children had data on all covariates at baseline as well as the Numbers Activity score at age 17. Of these, 294 (3.8%) had short stature at age 3. Regression models showed an association between short stature at age 3 and lower maths scores at age 17. Having short stature at age 3 was associated with a Numbers Activity score at age 17 that was 5% lower on average than those of children who had height SDS >  − 2. These associations were attenuated but remained significant after adjustment.

Discussion

In this sample of 12,536 UK children participating in the Millennium Cohort Study, there were associations between short stature (also termed stunting) and poorer performance in language testing from ages 3 to 11 years. Children with short stature had language scores around a quarter of a standard deviation below those with normal height. Whilst the association of short stature attenuated after adjustment for maternal, child and deprivation factors, it remained a significant predictor of poorer language development. In trajectory modelling describing four classes of performance, short stature was associated with a higher risk of being in the two low-performing classes and a lower risk of being in the high-performing class. After adjustment, children with short stature remained significantly more likely to be in the lowest-performing group and less likely to be in the high-score group. We did not find any interactions between time and short stature, which suggests children with short stature did not improve or worsen compared to their peers. However, we want to exercise caution in interpreting this result due to the high power needed for interaction effects. Children with short stature at age 3 years who had ‘caught up’ in height by age 5 years had better language test scores than children who still had short stature at 5 years of age, but there was still a decrement in language attainment compared to children who never had short stature. Finally, we found a relationship between short stature and language development at age 14 years, and mathematical ability at age 17 years, highlighting the long-term associations between early-life short stature and school performance.

In contrast to LMICs, few recent population-level studies in HICs have investigated the relationship between linear growth and cognition, and the existing evidence is conflicting. A cross-sectional relationship between height and cognition/intelligence/academic test scores has been identified in several studies including school-age children in HICs over the last century [61,62,63,64] and evidence from the US Fragile Families study has provided evidence from a HIC that the association between height and cognition is appreciable prior to school-age [65]. Other studies in HICs have found no association between height and cognition, although these studies had small sample sizes (n = 91 and n = 221) and both recruited from single sites [66, 67]. A 2004 review of the evidence of associations between short stature and cognition highlighted the overall low quality of the available evidence including common limitations of small sample sizes and selection bias, limiting the external validity of previous studies [68],

We found that children with height SDS ≤  − 1 and height SDS ≤  − 2 both had lower language scores than children with height SDS between 0 and 1; however, after adjustment, only those with height SDS ≤  − 2 had significantly lower scores. This frames the importance of different types of growth in this twenty-first-century cohort in a high-income setting: the association appears to be driven by the ‘tail’ of children with short stature rather than height per se. This is consistent with other large studies in the USA and UK. Data from the US National Health Examination Survey in the 1960s found the association between height and cognitive test scores was most pronounced for very short children; those with heights less than the fifth percentile had significantly lower IQ than controls [63]. The UK Wessex Growth Study of children born in the 1980s showed similarly that children with heights below the 3rd centile had significantly lower scores on measures of IQ (102.6 versus 108.6; p < 0.005), reading attainment, and basic number skills than controls. However, contrary to these and our findings, other studies have identified a continuous association between height SDS and later outcomes without an obvious inflection point at − 1 or − 2 SD, suggesting variance in growth patterns in different settings which merits further investigation [18, 65, 69].

We found that short stature at age 3 years was longitudinally associated with a deficit in language scores of around a quarter of a standard deviation. Comparisons with other studies are challenging because of differences in methodology, as well as the age of participants and the cognitive tests employed. Nevertheless, results from supplemental analyses are broadly consistent with a 2015 meta-analysis of data from 29 LMICs, which found that each unit increase in height SDS for children over 2 years old was associated with 0.09 SD (95% CI 0.05–0.12) increase in cognitive scores [18] and also with data from the US Fragile Families cohort of 3-year-old children born between 1998 and 2000, which demonstrated 5–10% of a SD increase in cognitive test score at age 3 per SD increase in height [65]. Comparison with effect sizes from intervention studies highlights the strength of the association between poor linear growth and impaired language development outcomes among children in this UK cohort. Programmes in LMICs and HICs ranging from delivery of single interventions, such as cash transfers, to complex multi-pronged early child development packages have reported effect sizes of between 0.1 and around 0.4 [70, 71].

Given the vital importance of the first 1000 days to linear growth and brain development [72], the question of whether catch-up growth and development are possible after this ‘window period’ has been the subject of debate. Evidence from multiple LMICs has demonstrated that whilst the first 1000 days are critical to cognitive, motor and social-emotional development, linear growth after 2 years of age appears either not associated or significantly more weakly associated with cognitive scores and academic outcomes [16, 18, 73,74,75,76,77]. The potential for catch-up development among children with short stature is important because without intervention, children with delayed cognitive development are more likely to fall behind throughout subsequent education and employment [78,79,80]. Using mixed effects linear models adjusted for maternal, child and deprivation factors, we found that although children with and without catch-up growth both had lower language development scores than children who did not have short stature, these were not significantly different from each other. However, children who had height-for-age ≤  − 2 at age 3 years, but > 2 at age 5 years had higher language development scores and a higher probability of being in the high scores trajectory group. We chose 5 years as the age to re-assess language scores, as the potential for ‘catch-up’ growth and development is thought to be greater in early life [81]. We considered the potential for some of these children to have ‘regressed to the mean’ rather than ‘caught-up’ [82]. However, the inclusion of all children who had short stature at age 3 years but not at age 5 years is a more conservative approach to estimate these effects.

The mechanisms for the association between linear growth in early life and downstream cognitive, developmental, educational and economic outcomes are not fully established. Moreover, much of the research interrogating the intermediary pathways has taken place in LMICs, so the findings of that body of research—especially relating to severe malnutrition, sanitation, infection burden and immune dysfunction—might not be generalisable. However, a recent analysis of national data in England identified a strong linear association between short stature and deprivation, and Table 1 demonstrates a similar pattern among MCS children [83]. Together, these results suggest that the well-documented interactions between poverty and growth in historical HIC cohorts and LMICs hold true in contemporary high-income settings. In the MCS cohort, short stature at age 3 years remained a predictor for impaired language development even after adjustment for several socioeconomic variables. This could represent a direct effect, residual confounding or a combination of both. We did not directly investigate mechanisms, but children with short stature in this cohort had lower birth weight, were less likely to have mothers with a high level of education, were more likely to be exposed to second-hand smoke, were less likely to have had any breastfeeding, had younger mothers and a lower (i.e. more deprived) index of multiple deprivation. Further research is required to investigate the pathways between growth, development and child poverty in both high-income and LMIC settings.

This study had several strengths including the relevance to policy and practice of a large national cohort of children born in the twenty-first century. This is also the first of the four national British birth cohort studies to have taken anthropometric measurements from all children at the age of 3 years and we investigated longitudinal associations up to the age of 17, which represents a follow-up to an older age than has been investigated by most earlier studies. The sampling strategy also overcomes a serious shortcoming of many previous studies in high-income countries, which have often been subject to referral bias. The study also had several limitations. The earliest height measurement in the MCS data was taken at age 3, and the most recently published data was from participants at age 17. We were therefore unable to assess linear growth patterns in the first 1000 days, during which period growth is most sensitive to the environment [84]. We were also unable to investigate whether the associations persist into adulthood. We adjusted for a large number of covariates, but the non-availability of some factors in the DAG (Additional file 1: Fig. 2) could have resulted in residual confounding. Furthermore, data on birth length were not available; whilst birth weight and birth length are correlated, there is some evidence that length is more directly associated with height in later life [85]. However, in practice, birth length measures are often inaccurate [86], and the correlation between birth weight and birth length could have resulted in collinearity if they had both been included in the models. We observed differences between our excluded and included samples, suggesting that those who were excluded from the current analyses had worse verbal and language development scores and higher prevalence of short stature, as well as being more likely to experience other adverse circumstances such as exposure to second-hand smoke, no breastfeeding and higher levels of deprivation. Although these children represent a small group, it is possible that we underestimated the relationship between short stature and language development, because of the socioeconomic vulnerability of the excluded group.

Conclusions

Whilst the observational nature of the study limits possible conclusions about any causal relationships between short stature and cognition, the strength of the longitudinal association after adjustment for socioeconomic confounders is striking. Short stature at age 3 years or younger could potentially serve as a marker of future risk of cognitive or educational problems and be used to identify children who would benefit from further assessment and early intervention. Such a marker could have several benefits over other predictive factors for impaired cognition in later life. Height is relatively simple and inexpensive to measure, interpretation is straightforward and linear growth monitoring in childhood is already embedded in public healthcare in most countries [87, 88]. A routine height measurement is also potentially less stigmatising than assessment for other markers of poverty. In contrast, many countries (including the UK) only measure children’s heights routinely at school entry, and further research is required to establish the feasibility, acceptability and utility of pre-school growth screening in high-income countries.

Since most children with short stature will not have impaired cognitive development, the challenge for a successful pre-school growth-screening programme is to sensitively identify the children who could benefit from early intervention without trading off specificity and causing harm through unnecessary investigation. Equally, it is important not to medicalise and stigmatise individual children’s ‘short but normal’ growth. Although short stature appears to be an important predictor of future cognition, it might not be sufficient to identify the children most in need. Further research is required to establish the variables which could be used in combination with linear growth to accurately identify children who would benefit from early intervention to support cognitive development.

Availability of data and materials

Data from the MCS are accessible on request from the UK Data Service.

Abbreviations

DAG:

Directed acyclic graph

HICs:

High-income countries

IMD:

Index of Multiple Deprivation

IQ:

Intelligence quotient

LMICs:

Low- and middle-income countries

MCS:

Millennium Cohort Study

References

  1. Local Burden of Disease Child Growth Failure Collaborators. Mapping child growth failure across low- and middle-income countries. Nature. 2020;577(7789):231-4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1038/s41586-019-1878-8.

  2. Orr J, Freer J, Morris JK, Hancock C, Walton R, Dunkel L, et al. Regional differences in short stature in England between 2006 and 2019: a cross-sectional analysis from the National Child Measurement Programme. PLoS Med. 2021;18(9):e1003760-e.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Hancock C, Bettiol S, Smith L. Socioeconomic variation in height: analysis of National Child Measurement Programme data for England. Arch Dis Child. 2016;101(5):422–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Teranishi H, Nakagawa H, Marmot M. Social class difference in catch up growth in a national British cohort. Arch Dis Child. 2001;84(3):218–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. The Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Crookston BT, Schott W, Cueto S, Dearden KA, Engle P, Georgiadis A, et al. Postinfancy growth, schooling, and cognitive achievement: Young Lives. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98(6):1555–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Kowalski AJ, Georgiadis A, Behrman JR, Crookston BT, Fernald LCH, Stein AD. Linear growth through 12 years is weakly but consistently associated with language and math achievement scores at age 12 years in 4 low- or middle-income countries. J Nutr. 2018;148(11):1852–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Fink G, Rockers PC. Childhood growth, schooling, and cognitive development: further evidence from the Young Lives study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;100(1):182–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Georgiadis A, Benny L, Crookston B, Duc L, Hermida P, Mani S, et al. Growth trajectories from conception through middle childhood and cognitive achievement at age 8 years: evidence from four low- and middle-income countries. SSM - Population Health. 2016;2:43–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Crookston BT, Penny ME, Alder SC, Dickerson TT, Merrill RM, Stanford JB, et al. Children who recover from early stunting and children who are not stunted demonstrate similar levels of cognition. J Nutr. 2010;140(11):1996–2001.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Poveda NE, Hartwig FP, Victora CG, Adair LS, Barros FC, Bhargava SK, et al. Patterns of growth in childhood in relation to adult schooling attainment and intelligence quotient in 6 birth cohorts in low- and middle-income countries: evidence from the Consortium of Health-Oriented Research in Transitioning Societies (COHORTS). J Nutr. 2021;151(8):2342–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Nahar B, Hossain M, Mahfuz M, Islam MM, Hossain MI, Murray-Kolb LE, et al. Early childhood development and stunting: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study in Bangladesh. Matern Child Nutr. 2020;16(1):e12864.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Berkman DS, Lescano AG, Gilman RH, Lopez SL, Black MM. Effects of stunting, diarrhoeal disease, and parasitic infection during infancy on cognition in late childhood: a follow-up study. Lancet. 2002;359(9306):564–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Grantham-McGregor SM. Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: prospective cohort study. Lancet. 2005;366(9499):1804–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Miller AC, Murray MB, Thomson DR, Arbour MC. How consistent are associations between stunting and child development? Evidence from a meta-analysis of associations between stunting and multidimensional child development in fifteen low- and middle-income countries. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(8):1339–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hamadani JD, Tofail F, Huda SN, Alam DS, Ridout DA, Attanasio O, et al. Cognitive deficit and poverty in the first 5 years of childhood in Bangladesh. Pediatrics. 2014;134(4):e1001–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Haile D, Nigatu D, Gashaw K, Demelash H. Height for age z score and cognitive function are associated with academic performance among school children aged 8–11 years old. Arch Public Health. 2016;74:17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Sudfeld CR, McCoy DC, Danaei G, Fink G, Ezzati M, Andrews KG, et al. Linear growth and child development in low- and middle-income countries: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2015;135(5):e1266–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Horta BL, Victora CG, de Mola CL, Quevedo L, Pinheiro RT, Gigante DP, et al. Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain in early life with human capital at 30 years of age. J Pediatr. 2017;182:85-91.e3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Gandhi M, Ashorn P, Maleta K, Teivaanmäki T, Duan X, Cheung YB. Height gain during early childhood is an important predictor of schooling and mathematics ability outcomes. Acta Paediatr. 2011;100(8):1113–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Tarleton JL, Haque R, Mondal D, Shu J, Farr BM, Petri WA Jr. Cognitive effects of diarrhea, malnutrition, and Entamoeba histolytica infection on school age children in Dhaka. Bangladesh Am J Trop Med Hyg. 2006;74(3):475–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kar BR, Rao SL, Chandramouli BA. Cognitive development in children with chronic protein energy malnutrition. Behav Brain Funct. 2008;4(1):31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh D, Wadsworth ME. Birthweight, postnatal growth and cognitive function in a national UK birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;31(2):342–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Pearce MS, Deary IJ, Young AH, Parker L. Growth in early life and childhood IQ at age 11 years: the Newcastle Thousand Families Study. Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34(3):673–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. University of London IoE, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. Millennium Cohort Study: first survey, 2001–2003. 13th Edition ed. London: UK Data Service; 2021.

  26. University of London IoE, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. Millennium Cohort Study: second survey, 2003–2005. London.

  27. 10th Edition ed. UK Data Service. London: 2021.

  28. University of London IoE, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. Millennium Cohort Study: third survey. London: 2006.

  29. 8th Edition ed. UK Data Service. London: 2021.

  30. University of London IoE, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. Millennium Cohort Study: fourth survey. London: 2008.

  31. 8th Edition ed. UK Data Service. London: 2021.

  32. University of London IoE, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. Millennium Cohort Study: fifth survey, 2012. 5th Edition ed. UK Data Service. London: 2021.

  33. University of London IoE, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. Millennium Cohort Study: sixth survey, 2015. 7th Edition ed. UK Data Service. London: 2020.

  34. University of London IoE, Centre for Longitudinal Studies. Millennium Cohort Study: seventh survey, 2018. 2nd Edition ed. London: UK Data Service; 2021.

  35. Joshi H, Fitzsimons E. The Millennium Cohort Study: the making of a multi-purpose resource for social science and policy. Longitudinal Life Course Studies. 2016;7(4):409–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Connelly R. Interpreting test scores. Institute of Education, Discussion Paper. 2013(2013/1).

  37. Barbuscia A, Mills MC. Cognitive development in children up to age 11 years born after ART—a longitudinal cohort study. Hum Reprod. 2017;32(7):1482–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Wright CM, Williams AF, Elliman D, Bedford H, Birks E, Butler G, et al. Using the new UK-WHO growth charts. BMJ. 2010;340:c1140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Shenkin SD, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Birth weight and cognitive ability in childhood: a systematic review. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(6):989–1013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Linsell L, Malouf R, Morris J, Kurinczuk JJ, Marlow N. Prognostic factors for poor cognitive development in children born very preterm or with very low birth weight: a systematic review. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(12):1162–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Caravale B, Tozzi C, Albino G, Vicari S. Cognitive development in low risk preterm infants at 3–4 years of life. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2005;90(6):F474–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Investigators M-EN. Early childhood cognitive development is affected by interactions among illness, diet, enteropathogens and the home environment: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study. BMJ Glob Health. 2018;3(4):e000752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Sacker A, Quigley MA, Kelly YJ. Breastfeeding and developmental delay: findings from the millennium cohort study. Pediatrics. 2006;118(3):e682–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Smith JM. Breastfeeding and language outcomes: a review of the literature. J Commun Disord. 2015;57:29–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hernández-Alava M, Popli G. Children’s development and parental input: evidence from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Demography. 2017;54(2):485–511.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Mensah FK, Kiernan KE. Parents’ mental health and children’s cognitive and social development: families in England in the Millennium Cohort Study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2010;45(11):1023–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Field T. Prenatal depression effects on early development: a review. Infant Behav Dev. 2011;34(1):1–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Srivastava P, Trinh TA. The effect of parental smoking on children’s cognitive and non-cognitive skills. Econ Hum Biol. 2021;41:100978.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Tearne JE. Older maternal age and child behavioral and cognitive outcomes: a review of the literature. Fertil Steril. 2015;103(6):1381–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Moulton V, Goodman A, Nasim B, Ploubidis GB, Gambaro L. Parental wealth and children’s cognitive ability, mental, and physical health: evidence from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Child Dev. 2021;92(1):115–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Tomalski P, Moore DG, Ribeiro H, Axelsson EL, Murphy E, Karmiloff-Smith A, et al. Socioeconomic status and functional brain development - associations in early infancy. Dev Sci. 2013;16(5):676–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Jelenkovic A, Yokoyama Y, Sund R, Hur YM, Harris JR, Brandt I, et al. Associations between birth size and later height from infancy through adulthood: an individual based pooled analysis of 28 twin cohorts participating in the CODATwins project. Early Hum Dev. 2018;120:53–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Haas JD, Campirano F. Interpopulation variation in height among children 7 to 18 years of age. Food Nutr Bull. 2006;27(4 Suppl Growth Standard):S212-23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Finch BK, Beck AN. Socio-economic status and z-score standardized height-for-age of U.S.-born children (ages 2–6). Econ Hum Biol. 2011;9(3):272–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Prendergast AJ, Humphrey JH. The stunting syndrome in developing countries. Paediatr Int Child Health. 2014;34(4):250–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Li L, Power C. Influences on childhood height: comparing two generations in the 1958 British birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2004;33(6):1320–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Wright CM, Booth IW, Buckler JMH, Cameron N, Cole TJ, Healy MJR, et al. Growth reference charts for use in the United Kingdom. Arch Dis Child. 2002;86(1):11–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. UK Government. What qualification levels mean 2021 [Available from: https://www.gov.uk/what-different-qualification-levels-mean/list-of-qualification-levels.

  59. Bolton P. Education: historical statistics. London: House of Commons Library; 2012;27.

  60. Population by Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD), England, 2001 to 2019 - Office for National Statistics; 2020. Available from: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/populationandmigration/populationestimates/adhocs/12386populationbyindexofmultipledeprivationimdengland2001to2019.

  61. Hansen K, Johnson J, Calderwood L, Mostafa T, Platt L, Rosenberg R, Smith K. Millennium cohort study: a guide to the datasets. Centre for Longitudinal Studies. In CLS Working Papers Series (CLS Working Papers Series). 2014.

  62. Nagin DS, Jones BL, Passos VL, Tremblay RE. Group-based multi-trajectory modeling. Stat Methods Med Res. 2018;27(7):2015–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Cole TJ. Do growth chart centiles need a face lift? BMJ. 1994;308(6929):641–2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Stephen MD, Varni JW, Limbers CA, Yafi M, Heptulla RA, Renukuntla VS, et al. Health-related quality of life and cognitive functioning in pediatric short stature: comparison of growth-hormone-naïve, growth-hormone-treated, and healthy samples. Eur J Pediatr. 2011;170(3):351–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Porter WT. The physical basis of precocity and dullness. 1893. Nutrition. 1992;8(4):285–97 (discussion 98).

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Wilson DM, Hammer LD, Duncan PM, Dornbusch SM, Ritter PL, Hintz RL, et al. Growth and intellectual development. Pediatrics. 1986;78(4):646–50.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Weinberg WA, Dietz SG, Penick EC, McAlister WH. Intelligence, reading achievement, physical size and social class. A study of St. Louis Caucasian boys aged 8-0 to 9-6 years, attending regular schools. J Pediatr. 1974;85(4):482–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Case A, Paxson C. Stature and status: height, ability, and labor market outcomes. J Polit Econ. 2008;116(3):499–532.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Gale CR, O’Callaghan FJ, Godfrey KM, Law CM, Martyn CN. Critical periods of brain growth and cognitive function in children. Brain. 2004;127(Pt 2):321–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Pollitt E, Mueller W. The relation of growth to cognition in a well-nourished preschool population. Child Dev. 1982;53(5):1157–63.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  71. Wheeler PG, Bresnahan K, Shephard BA, Lau J, Balk EM. Short stature and functional impairment: a systematic review. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004;158(3):236–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Perumal N, Bassani DG, Roth DE. Use and misuse of stunting as a measure of child health. J Nutr. 2018;148(3):311–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Spittle A, Orton J, Anderson PJ, Boyd R, Doyle LW. Early developmental intervention programmes provided post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairment in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015;11:CD005495.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Schady N, Behrman J, Araujo MC, Azuero R, Bernal R, Bravo D, et al. Wealth gradients in early childhood cognitive development in five Latin American countries. J Hum Resour. 2015;50(2):446–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Victora CG, Adair L, Fall C, Hallal PC, Martorell R, Richter L, et al. Maternal and child undernutrition: consequences for adult health and human capital. Lancet. 2008;371(9609):340–57.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Poveda NE, Hartwig FP, Victora CG, Adair LS, Barros FC, Bhargava SK, et al. Patterns of growth in childhood in relation to adult schooling attainment and intelligence quotient in 6 birth cohorts in low- and middle-income countries: evidence from the Consortium of Health-Oriented Research in Transitioning Societies (COHORTS). J Nutr. 2021;151(8):2342–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Hoddinott J, Behrman JR, Maluccio JA, Melgar P, Quisumbing AR, Ramirez-Zea M, et al. Adult consequences of growth failure in early childhood. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98(5):1170–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  78. Baptista Menezes AM, Oliveira PD, Wehrmeister FC, Anselmi L, Gonçalves H, Martorell R, et al. Associations between growth from birth to 18 years, intelligence, and schooling in a Brazilian cohort. Am J Clin Nutr. 2020;112(1):187–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Pongcharoen T, Ramakrishnan U, DiGirolamo AM, Winichagoon P, Flores R, Singkhornard J, et al. Influence of prenatal and postnatal growth on intellectual functioning in school-aged children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(5):411–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Martorell R, Horta BL, Adair LS, Stein AD, Richter L, Fall CH, et al. Weight gain in the first two years of life is an important predictor of schooling outcomes in pooled analyses from five birth cohorts from low- and middle-income countries. J Nutr. 2010;140(2):348–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  81. M M. Strategic review of health inequalities in England post-2010. 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Jane C, Duncan T. Early test scores, socioeconomic status and future outcomes. Cambridge: National Bureau of Economic Research; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Magnusson PK, Rasmussen F, Gyllensten UB. Height at age 18 years is a strong predictor of attained education later in life: cohort study of over 950,000 Swedish men. Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35(3):658–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Desmond C, Casale D. Catch-up growth in stunted children: definitions and predictors. PLoS One. 2017;12(12):e0189135.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Cameron N, Preece MA, Cole TJ. Catch-up growth or regression to the mean? Recovery from stunting revisited. Am J Hum Biol. 2005;17(4):412–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Orr J, Freer J, Morris JK, Hancock C, Walton R, Dunkel L, et al. Regional differences in short stature in England between 2006 and 2019: a cross-sectional analysis from the National Child Measurement Programme. PLoS Med. 2021;18(9):e1003760.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Martorell R, Zongrone A. Intergenerational influences on child growth and undernutrition. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2012;26(Suppl 1):302–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Sørensen HT, Sabroe S, Rothman KJ, Gillman M, Steffensen FH, Fischer P, et al. Birth weight and length as predictors for adult height. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;149(8):726–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Wood AJ, Raynes-Greenow CH, Carberry AE, Jeffery HE. Neonatal length inaccuracies in clinical practice and related percentile discrepancies detected by a simple length-board. J Paediatr Child Health. 2013;49(3):199–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Savage MO, Backeljauw PF, Calzada R, Cianfarani S, Dunkel L, Koledova E, et al. Early detection, referral, investigation, and diagnosis of children with growth disorders. Horm Res Paediatr. 2016;85(5):325–32.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  91. de Onis M, Wijnhoven TM, Onyango AW. Worldwide practices in child growth monitoring. J Pediatr. 2004;144(4):461–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study was funded by Barts Charity (grant MRC0219). AJP is funded by Wellcome (grant 108065/Z/15/Z). HS is funded by NIHR (grant NIHR300098). The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JF and JO share joint first authorship. JO, JF and AP conceptualised the study, reviewed the literature, planned the analysis and wrote the original draft. JO and JM conducted the formal analysis and verified the underlying data. AP, JF, JO, JM, HS, LD and RW interpreted results, reviewed and edited the manuscript. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joseph Freer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval for the MCS was granted by the NHS Multi-Centre Research Ethics Committee (MREC): MREC03/2/022 (3 years), 05/MRE02/46 (5 years), 11/YH/0203 (11 years), 13/LO/1786 (14 years) and 17/NE/0341 (17 years). Integrated Research Application System (IRAS) approval for the current analysis was not required. Written informed consent was obtained from parents, and where appropriate, children and young people prior to data collection in the MCS. More information on the informed consent procedures is here: https://cls.ucl.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/MCS-Ethical-Approval-and-Consent-2019.pdf. This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Contains the study’s sampling flowchart; a Directed Acyclic Graph; details of our missing data handling and imputation strategy; and details of our supplemental analyses.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Freer, J., Orr, J., Morris, J.K. et al. Short stature and language development in the United Kingdom: a longitudinal analysis of children from the Millennium Cohort Study. BMC Med 20, 468 (2022). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12916-022-02680-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12916-022-02680-y

Keywords